Skip to main content
Advertisement
Browse Subject Areas
?

Click through the PLOS taxonomy to find articles in your field.

For more information about PLOS Subject Areas, click here.

  • Loading metrics

Understanding the reminiscence bump: A systematic review

  • Khadeeja Munawar ,

    Contributed equally to this work with: Khadeeja Munawar, Sara K. Kuhn

    Roles Conceptualization, Data curation, Formal analysis, Project administration, Writing – original draft

    Affiliations Department of Psychology, Jeffrey Cheah School of Medicine and Health Sciences, Monash University Malaysia, Jalan Lagoon Selatan, Bandar Sunway, Subang Jaya, Selangor, Malaysia, Department of Psychology, University of Wah, Wah Cantt, Pakistan

  • Sara K. Kuhn ,

    Contributed equally to this work with: Khadeeja Munawar, Sara K. Kuhn

    Roles Writing – review & editing

    Affiliation Department of Teaching and Learning, College of Education and Human Development, University of North Dakota, Grand Forks, North Dakota, United States of America

  • Shamsul Haque

    Roles Conceptualization, Funding acquisition, Project administration, Supervision, Writing – review & editing

    shamsul@monash.edu

    Affiliation Department of Psychology, Jeffrey Cheah School of Medicine and Health Sciences, Monash University Malaysia, Jalan Lagoon Selatan, Bandar Sunway, Subang Jaya, Selangor, Malaysia

Abstract

One of the most consistently observed phenomena in autobiographical memory research is the reminiscence bump: a tendency for middle-aged and elderly people to access more personal memories from approximately 10–30 years of age. This systematic review (PROSPERO 2017:CRD42017076695) aimed to synthesize peer-reviewed literature pertaining to the reminiscence bump. The researchers conducted searches in nine databases for studies published between the date of inception of each database and the year 2017. Keywords used included: reminiscence, bump, peak, surge, blip, reminiscence effect, and reminiscence component. Sixty-eight quantitative studies, out of 523, met the inclusion criteria. The researchers implemented a thematic analytic technique for data extraction. Four main themes were generated: methods of memory activation/instruction for life scripts, types of memory/life scripts recalled, location of the reminiscence bump, and theoretical accounts for the bump. The two prevailing methods of memory activation implemented were the cuing method and important memories method. Three types of memories/life scripts were recalled: personal/autobiographical memory, memories for public events, and life script events. The findings illustrate differing temporal periods for the bump: approximately 10–30 years for memories for important events, approximately 5–30 years for memories that were induced by word cues, and 6–39 years for studies using life scripts. In explaining the bump, the narrative/identity account and cultural life script account received the most support.

Introduction

When examining the life span distribution of autobiographical memories (AMs), three phenomena are revealed. The first is childhood amnesia, or the limited recollection of AMs from a very young age, which is present in the life span retrieval curve as a steadily rising function between 0–8 years of age [1]. The second, the recency effect, dictates that memories recalled by most individuals are of recent events, and the frequency of these memories decline gradually [2]. Lastly, the reminiscence bump—also known as “the bump”—enhances memory recall from approximately 10–30 years of age by people over the age of 30 [3], and is considered one of the most robust findings in AM research [4]. As the reminiscence bump features deviation from the standard forgetting curve and forgetting functions [2, 3], it is recognized as a peculiar phenomenon and defining feature of AM [2, 3]. The reminiscence bump concept is included in most introductory psychology textbooks due to its significance to AM and the field of cognitive psychology [57].

The distribution of AMs across the adult life span is often studied through two major types of cueing techniques: the word cuing method and the important memories method [8]. There are various other methods of activating memories, some of them are; writing home diaries, free-recall of public and private items of news, and answering factual, semantic, general-knowledge, multiple-choice questions about the Academy Awards, the World Series, and current events [911]. The word cuing method, originally developed by Francis Galton, was later modified by Crovitz and Schiffman [12, 13]. In this technique, participants retrieve and report memories in response to word cues commonly used in everyday conversation [1417]. The word cuing method was used rigorously in investigating personal memories during the 1970s and 1980s [12, 1820]. In the important memories method, participants are instructed to retrieve and report the most important memories from their life [8, 15, 21, 22], especially the vivid ones [2325].

A significant amount of research emerging in the last two decades, claims that the previously found reminiscence bump in AM also extends to public events [26]. Research shows that public events which occurred during adolescence or early adulthood, approximately from the age of 12 to 29 years, are preferentially recalled [27]. This phenomenon is assessed through two major methodologies: the first asks participants to name significant events from recent history [28], and the second assesses participants’ level of factual knowledge of specific events [11].

Researchers propose various theoretical accounts to explain the reminiscence bump [8], including the: cognitive account, cognitive abilities account, cultural life script account, and narrative/identity account. The cognitive account postulates that it is simply the novelty of many events occurring in the second and third decades of life that is the major factor leading to enhanced memory recall from this period [29]. According to the cognitive abilities account, people become better equipped to learn, process, and retain information as they move into adolescence and early adulthood due to the maturation of the brain, which leads to maximal cognitive and neurological functioning [30]. According to the cultural life script account, individuals recall more events from the second and third decades of life because of cultural prescriptions and expectations present in the life script [3133].

The narrative/identity account states that events occurring during adolescence and early adulthood are vital to the development of the individual’s adult identity. It is this time when an individual engages in activities and relationships that define who the person will finally become, and how they narrate the stories of their lives [11, 24, 34, 35]. By the time an individual reaches this period of life, the effect of novel experiences on long-term memory, recognition, self-identity, and the development and consolidation of goals, have typically been demonstrated [36, 37]. Experiences acquired during this period are integrated into an individual’s lifelong narratives, thus they are more easily recalled later in life. The critical role of early adulthood AMs in identity formation is illustrated in neuropsychological and developmental research [38, 39]. The working self is viewed as playing a major role in organizing AMs; and events during this critical period are used as identity markers for the remainder of life each time AMs are reconstructed [40]. Broadly, the self (or identity) is conceptualized as a multidimensional and complicated set of self-related processes and schema [36, 41, 42].

The research identifies two components of the reminiscence bump: one relating to social identity (i.e. AMs corresponding chiefly to public events individuals experienced when ages 10 to 19 years old), and the other relating to personal identity (i.e. AMs corresponding to personal events that happened between the ages of 20–29) [10]. While social identity develops, individuals associate themselves with specific cultural, social, political, and/or religious groups with whom they have similar goals and desires [43]. Alternatively, during the development of personal identity, desires and goals towards establishing interaction with significant others and forming intimate relationships are developed [36]. The enhanced recollection of social events results in the formation of a reminiscence bump for the ages of 10–19 years, while developing close personal relationships results in a bump for the ages of 20–29 years [10].

Cultural life scripts are stereotypical episodes comprising multiple events in a specific order, with every event allowing succeeding events to occur [44]. They are a series of events which occur in a particular sequence and characterize a prototypical life span in a certain culture [3133]. The scripts have slots with specific conditions for what is allowed to fill them [45]. The slots in cultural life scripts are culturally significant transitional events likely to occur in a prototypical life span in a certain culture [46]. According to one study, an important life script characteristic is that it represents a culturally shared part of our semantic knowledge; not the outcome of a few personal experiences [32]. Another research study opposes this finding, reporting that cultural life scripts are not part of our shared semantic knowledge [47].

The cultural life script account is based on observation; as reported by Neugarten, Moore and Lowe (1965): Certain age norms are present in every society which organize the expectations, and structure the behaviour, of individuals [46]. There are prescriptive timetables in every culture for the arrangement of significant life events (e.g., finish school, get a job, get married, and have the first child) and the individuals of that particular culture are aware of these age norms [48]. Individuals also manage their own timing for the events on these timetables, and assess if they are achieving significant events earlier, or later, than anticipated [49]. The mechanisms underlying the bump for public events may be different than the mechanisms underlying the bump in autobiographical memory.

The earlier review papers summarized the studies on retention function, reanalyzed the previous findings, reviewed the temporal location of the bump according to different cueing methods, and assessed the current theoretical accounts of the bump in light of the temporal locations of the bump [2, 4, 8, 26]. These studies presented different bump periods for different methods of memory activation. The past reviews summarized the empirical evidence on the bump from non-clinical sample and excluded findings from the clinical samples as well as immigrants as they were interest in "the location of the bump in the general population" (p.67, Koppel & Berntsen, 2015). The differences in location of the bump with respect to cuing methods has already been shown in the past research studies [15, 16]. However, the mechanisms underlying the bump for autobiographical memory activated by different cuing methods (e.g., word cuing method and important memory method) might be different as well as the mechanisms underlying the bump for different types of responses (autobiographical memory vs. Life script events) could also be different.

As the bump is one of the most robust findings in autobiographical memory research, the present review paper added to the empirical body of literature on the bump and attempted to take a step a little further by including and reporting past studies’ findings on: general population, clinical samples, immigrants, or any other samples. Furthermore, no restriction was applied on methods of memory activation and research studies assessing the reminiscence bump through methods other than word cuing methods and important methods were also screened for eligibility. The last review paper on bump was published in 2015, therefore, this review was conducted with the belief that more recent and latest findings from the studies could be assessed and summarized.

The authors attempted to present a thorough summary of all the existing primary research studies on the bump, tried to establish the state of existing knowledge and reviewed the bump for various types of memories apart from autobiographical memories, for instance, flashbulb memories and memories public/private events. The authors developed a clearly defined, predetermined eligibility and relevance criteria for including research studies; the methodology was reproducible, transparent and systematic; carried out a meticulous search to identify all suitable studies; assessed quality of included studies, and systematically synthesized all the evidence in the form of figures and tables. The authors tried to limit selection bias and random error which have been found to mislead the reviews [50, 51], and attempted to present a reliable summary of the existing knowledge.

In the existing literature, there is currently no systematic review on the reminiscence bump which implements the guidelines proposed in the Preferred Reporting Items for Systematic Reviews and Meta-Analyses (PRISMA) statement [52]. However, a general review of the location of the reminiscence bump across different methods of memory activation provides additional references and descriptions [8]. Although there are several theoretical accounts for the bump for various kinds of memories, and all receive some support in the literature, it is necessary to investigate the relative plausibility of each account. The temporal location of the bump is not presented consistently in studies using varied methods of memory activation. The mechanisms underlying the bump may be different across different memory domains and types of memory assessments.

Discovering the most likely temporal location of the reminiscence bump is one of the authors’ aims in conducting the systematic literature review presented in this paper. Another goal of the researchers is to ascertain which theoretical account for the reminiscence bump received the most overarching support, by examining the literature for reported temporal locations of the bump in relation to the methods used to activate different types of memories in participants from various countries.

Method

The authors implemented the PRISMA statement guidelines in designing this systematic review [52]. After the researchers developed the review protocol, they registered the protocol in PROSPERO (International prospective register of systematic reviews; please see S1 File) prior to the commencement of the review (registration number CRD42017076695) [53].

Eligibility criteria

Eligible articles were required to present original research on the bump from qualitative, experimental, quasi-experimental, non-experimental, observational, or mixed-method studies. Neither language of published article, nor sample age group was limited. Only articles published in peer-reviewed or refereed journals were selected. Grey literature and articles that did not mention the reminiscence bump or its synonyms in their titles or abstracts, were excluded. No restriction was imposed on date of publication to allow for a comprehensive background on—and theoretical progression for—the reminiscence bump over time, as presented in the research.

Systematic search strategy

The researchers conducted a systematic search to locate primary source articles. Synonymic keywords searched in each database, using the Boolean OR operator [54] and wildcard features (e.g. placing an asterisk at the end of a root word to account for a variety of word endings), included: reminiscence*, bump, peak, surge, blip, reminiscence effect*, and reminiscence component (please see S1 Table). The search strategy combined these synonymic keywords (with OR) to search the following 9 databases for articles published, or added, to the databases between the date of inception of each database and 2017: Ovid MEDLINE, Ovid Embase, Ovid Emcare, CINAHL Plus (EBSCOhost), Proquest Central, PsycInfo, Scopus, Pubmed, and ScienceDirect (please see S2 Table).

The researchers retrieved a total of 523 records through this search strategy. They then “hand searched” the 523 articles’ reference lists to obtain further relevant studies, yielding 47 additional citations (Fig 1). After the researchers removed duplicates, 261 articles remained.

thumbnail
Fig 1. PRISMA flow diagram showing process of study selection for inclusion in systematic review.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0208595.g001

The 261 article records were imported into the EndNote reference/citation manager and screened for eligibility/inclusion in the review. The researchers screened titles and abstracts for inclusion criteria, removing 181 studies that did not meet the criteria and retaining 80 full-text articles to assess for eligibility. After assessing the 80 full-text articles, 12 articles were excluded for not meeting inclusion criteria (e.g. not published in peer reviewed journals, or format was a brief report and not a research article). The researchers hand searched the remaining 68 articles’ references for relevant articles. No additional articles were included from this search. The final group of 68 studies was assessed for methodological quality, after which the researchers performed data extraction and synthesis.

Methodological assessment

The 68 quantitative studies were assessed for quality using the 14 criteria developed by Kmet, Lee, and Cook [55]. No qualitative or mixed-method studies were present. An overall rating (from 0 to 1) was assigned to every study; higher numerical ratings indicated higher quality. Previous systematic reviews employing the QualSyst quality assessment protocol required a minimum threshold score of 0.55 for study inclusion [56]. Other reviews included studies falling within the range of 0.74 to 0.91[57]. The lowest quality rating of studies included in this review was determined to be 0.54, therefore all studies were considered eligible for inclusion. To minimize the risk of bias, two reviewers worked independently to screen studies and extract and synthesize data. Disagreements were settled by applying the 14 criteria [55] (please see S3 Table).

Data collection and extraction

The researchers used forms to extracted the data for retrieving relevant information to assess the aims of the review [58]. They placed extracted information under appropriate sections corresponding to: author, year, country; study objective; sample size (N), and findings (Table 1)

thumbnail
Table 1. Summary of studies about the reminiscence bump 1 (N = 68).

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0208595.t001

Data synthesis

The researchers employed a narrative synthesis approach to abridge extracted study data. Narrative synthesis was deemed appropriate as it allows for both the synthesis of findings from several studies that use different research designs with varying characteristics of samples, and the application of an overall meaning to the data [115]. Through extraction, formulation (via a data extraction chart), and translation of the data into narrative summaries, the researchers utilized all accessible data. The initial stage of data familiarization occurred through the repeated systematic review of each research article. Formatting initial codes resulted in data refinement and the generation of themes from the data. Before allocating descriptive terms, themes were further refined so that the crux of all themes could be captured. These themes assisted in generating an analytic narrative during the final stage of report writing. Driven by the narrative synthesis approach, the data synthesis stage of this systematic review achieved the researchers’ goals of effectively drawing out—and giving meaning to—pertinent data from the research articles (please see S4 Table).

Results

Temporal and geographical representation of studies

The locations of the 68 studies varied: 19 in the U.S.; 9 in Denmark; 8 in the Netherlands; 7 in the United Kingdom; 4 in both Turkey and Japan; 3 in Germany, Canada and France, respectively; and 2 in Australia. One study took place in Bangladesh, Austria, Malaysia, and Trinidad and Tobago, respectively. Two studies included samples from more than one country: the first from Japan, Bangladesh, England, and China; and the second from the Netherlands and the United States (please see S1 Fig). Dates of published studies ranged from 1988 to 2017: 13 studies from 1988 to 1999, 29 studies from 2000 to 2010, and 26 studies from 2011 to 2017 (please see S2 Fig).

Research study themes

The articles selected for review comprised 68 quantitative studies (N = 68; see Fig 2).

thumbnail
Fig 2. Summary of themes and sub-themes derived from these studies.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0208595.g002

Four main themes emerged after reviewing these studies on the reminiscence bump: (a) methods of memory activation/instruction for life scripts, (b) types of memory /life scripts recalled, (c) location of the reminiscence bump, and (c) theoretical accounts for the bump. These four themes evolved from 13 sub-themes.

Method of memory activation/Instruction for life scripts.

The researchers used a variety of methods to recall memories or life events. Most studies implemented more than one recall method across a number of different populations.

Important memories method. Previous research consists of asking participants to report their important memories. Several studies ask participants about their most positive and negative, or most important and traumatic experiences, or important and surprising memories, used emotional cues (i.e. positive and negative), and asked participants to recall important positive and negative, or surprising positive and negative, events [22, 33, 35, 59, 72, 73, 100, 104, 107]. Furthermore, the past research studies focused on: (a) memories that are a vital component of life story, (b) life history timeline and significant life events narrative for the description of three events, (c) free narrative of life history about important life events and word cues, (d) descriptions of three self-defining memories, (e) Reminiscence Functions Scale and vivid memories that are important landmarks or turning points in life, (f) the Life Story Questionnaire for listing 15 personally important events or experiences, and (g) free narratives of life history; important life events [6870, 108, 111, 113]. Two questionnaire-based studies instructed participants to report self-defining “I am” statements which were later used in recalling memories as well as a life event list for gathering distributions of positive and negative events [91, 93].

Word cuing method. A number of research studies use cuing methods for collecting memories, for instance odor cues, emotional cues and words cues. However, most of the studies used word cues for activating memories. The type of cues used, and their number, varies from study to study, and some studies use more than one cue recall method: (a) using word cues (i.e. emotional, emotion-provoking, and neutral); (b) instructing children to write future life stories using 10 word cues; (c) using both odor cues and word label cues; (d) reading novels and then implementing a cued recall task; (e) reporting important word-cue memories; (f) using 50 word cues for AMs; (g) employing 15 word cues pertaining to common locations, objects, positive emotions, negative emotions, and significant others; (h) Associative Memory Questionnaire with 18 word cues, (i) modified Autobiographical Memory Test having a list of 16 word cues; and (j) questionnaires implementing both the word cues and important memories method [17, 6062, 64, 88, 89, 96, 114, 116].

Several studies assess life events through: (a) the use of various emotional cues to elicit and record events in the course of a typical and hypothetical person, and (b) verbal reporting of events from personal lives and diaries. Cued recall methods include: 10 word cues, 40 word cues for obtaining specific memories, and 20 ambiguous and 20 unambiguous single names [24, 75, 109]. A few studies use the Galton-Crovitz cueing method and Robinson word cuing technique [30, 7678, 81, 82, 86, 105, 106]. The number of word cues used to elicit memories range from 15–124 [15, 16, 74].

Life Scripts. The studies on life scripts used various instructions to explore the reminiscence bump. For instance, these studies assessed the reminiscence bump through: (a) asking participants to report important events likely to occur in the life of a newborn or an elderly person; (b) the life script questionnaire; and (c) the expected timing of the public event [32, 71, 84, 87, 101].

Other methods. Other memory recall methods that were not associated with the three methods already discussed, were found in some studies (e.g., dream diaries, the evaluation of participants’ reactions to nostalgic advertising, a modified version of timeline methods for recording specific memories, issues surrounding the Academy Awards, World Series, and current events, life regrets’ content and chronology, and free recall events having public or private nature[911, 63, 6567, 85, 110]. Adults were interviewed to discover their: recounted and oral life stories; specific autobiographical lifetime events; three favorite books, movies, and records, or the five best football players; and memories using the Yearly News Memory Test (YNMT) comprising 30 open-ended and multiple-choice questions [79, 80, 83, 97, 99, 102, 103, 112]. A few studies used music clips and instructed participants to imagine music as a means to access associated memories [90, 92, 94, 98].

Types of memory/life scripts recalled.

The 68 research studies elicited a variety of responses in types of memories activated.

Personal /autobiographical memories. One study elicited personal future life stories of children, and events from those stories [60]. In studies where participants reported autobiographical events for various types of cues, retrieved memories were from personal pasts [15, 16, 30, 33, 59, 6163, 69,74, 75, 7678, 81, 86, 89, 90, 92, 93, 96, 98, 104106, 109, 114]. The following were elicited in various studies: personal memories of adults spanning their life course, meaningful life events, life stories from across participants' life spans, lists of life events for AMs and collective memories, life-lines for both past and future events, and self-defining AMs [17, 22, 35, 66, 70, 72, 73, 91, 95, 97, 99, 102, 103, 107, 108, 110113]. One study collected participants’ personal specific memories and specific memories related to 70- and 80-year-old hypothetical cases [68].

Life scripts. These studies asked participants about important events likely to occur in the life of a newborn and important events a newborn or an elderly person would experience during his/her lifetime [32, 71]. A few research studies investigated original and modified versions of the life script questionnaire, and probed cultural expectations for the expected timing of the public event [84, 87, 101].

Public events. A number of research studies focused on memories for important public and private news items, as well as public events. [10, 80, 81]. In another study, authors elicited both autobiographical and public events [88].

Other memory types. Other research studies focused on curves of forgetting after memorizing a 10-chapter autobiographical novel, flashbulb memories, AM recall, and memories from the lives of participants’ parents [24, 64]. Some studies investigated dreams of older adults’ life regrets; flashbulb memories; reactions to nostalgic advertisements; and responses to the World Series, academy awards, and current events [9, 11, 65, 67, 85]. And in other studies, researchers asked participants to report three favorite books, movies, and records as well as the five best sports players of all time [79, 83, 94, 100].

Location of the bump.

The temporal range of the bump varied along with the method of memory activation and presence or absence of a mental health issue (see Table 2).

thumbnail
Table 2. Method of memory activation/life scripts and bump range.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0208595.t002

Important memories method. Research studies using this method revealed the location of the bump to be from a minimum of 10 years to a maximum of 40 years of age, and some studies also showed a more localized peak from ages 16 to 20 years of age [33, 35, 59, 68, 93, 100, 104, 107, 113]. Findings of another study did not show the bump for AM distribution, however, a bump appeared for the life script distribution as a result of suppressing typical life events [69]. Three age intervals corresponded to the bump period (i.e. 16–20, 21–25, and 26–30 years) [69]. The responses to a newborn questionnaire demonstrated a considerably large bump for positive events in the third decade of life, a very small bump for negative events in the second decade of life, and a clear bump for positive events between 10 to 30 years of age [71]. The range of the bumps for positive, negative, expected, unexpected events, and significant life events narrative were between 16 to 30 years and 20–29 years of ages, respectively [22, 70, 72, 73, 91]. A few studies compared the bumps of control groups and patients with schizophrenia and found different results for both groups: 15–19 and 16–25 years for patients with schizophrenia; 20–24 and 21–25 years for control groups [108, 111].

Word cuing method. Using these methods, the distribution of the future life stories of Danish children illustrated a bump in young adulthood, and this bump consisted of life-script events [60]. Studies discovered bumps from a minimum of 6 years of age to a maximum of 35 years of age [11, 15, 16, 24, 30, 61, 62, 7476, 78, 81, 82, 88, 89, 95, 106, 109, 114]. One study, using a novel cuing method, revealed two bumps: The first bump appeared when the protagonist of the story was 22 years old, and the second occurred when she was in her 50s and undergoing significant life changes [64]. Studies revealed the timing of the bump to be between the ages of 10 to 30, with peaks occurring between 15–18 for men, 13–14 for women, 5–13 for Japanese adults, and from 5–30 for personal and collective memories; a peak in recall occurred between 10–19 years of age for public items and 20–29 years of age for private items, and a bump between 10 to 30 years of age [17, 77, 78, 86, 105].

Life scripts. The studies on life scripts revealed varying locations of the bump. These studies revealed the bumps of positive and negative events from a minimum of 6 to a maximum of 39 years of age [32, 71, 84, 87, 101].

Other methods. A study using a heterogeneous sample from five countries (i.e. Japan, Bangladesh, U.K., China and the U.S.) revealed that more than 50% of memories were recalled from the ages of 10–30 [63]. Studies also showed a bump in late adolescence to early adulthood as well as a bump between 15 to 24 years of age [9, 11, 65, 67, 79, 85, 92, 94, 99]. Two studies, using a life history timeline method and a “life-line” interview method, discovered the bump between 10–30 and 10–40 years of age, respectively [66, 110]. A study using cues explored memories of younger and older Bangladeshi individuals: Younger adults showed a bump between 10–30 years of age, whereas an older group revealed a second bump between 35–55 years of age [62]. The bump corresponding to ages of immigrants at the time of immigration, occurred between 6–30 and 10–25 years of age [80, 97, 98, 102, 103]. A study using free recall flashbulb memories from personal lives reported a bump between 10 to 30 years of age [67]. A study conducted on patients with schizophrenia revealed bump between 16–25 years for patients and 21–25 for controls [112].

Theoretical accounts for the bump.

There are a variety of theoretical accounts for the bump, and each account has received varied levels of support in the research. The narrative/identity account is fully supported by the findings of seven studies [9, 10, 35, 36, 62, 70, 93], and partially supported by the findings of six studies [24, 72, 76, 108, 111, 112]. Two studies illustrated complete support for the cultural life script account [33, 71], while ten studies showed partial support [32, 60, 75, 87, 89, 100103, 114], and two studies demonstrated no support at all [59, 84].

Four studies supported the “life story” account [22, 66, 73, 105], three studies supported differential encoding and differential sampling [78, 79, 92], one study supported the “life-span perspective” [110], one study supported the cognitive account [97], and two studies showed some support for the “biological-maturational” account [30, 74]. Several research studies supported more than one theoretical account for the bump: the cultural life script and “novelty” accounts [64]; the narrative/identity and cultural life script accounts [65, 68, 107]; the narrative/identity, cognitive and “maturational” accounts [67, 96, 109]; the cognitive, narrative/identity, cultural life script, and life story accounts [69, 94]; the cognitive, narrative/identity, and cultural life script accounts [80]; the biological, narrative/identity, and cultural life script accounts [83]; and the cognitive and narrative/identity accounts [11].

Discussion

Despite the wealth of evidence existing on aspects of the reminiscence bump that present when using different methods for activating memories, there is a limited understanding of which theoretical account—or accounts—offers the best explanation for the bump, and the reasons for variation in the exact location of the bump. The aim of this systematic review is to establish a current evidence base concerning the understanding and formation of the bump, and to add to the existing body of literature on AMs, other kinds of memories and the reminiscence bump.

Summary of study findings

The results of this systematic review on the reminiscence bump are based on the analysis of 68 selected studies retrieved from 9 scientific databases and screened by the researchers. The results reveal that methods for activating memories/instruction for life scripts include the important memories method [33, 69, 100], word cuing method [17, 60, 61, 64, 68, 114], life scripts [32, 71, 84, 87, 101], and other heterogeneous methods [72, 73, 79, 80, 83, 97, 99, 102, 103]. A variety of responses were elicited from the participants including: AMs [15, 16, 30, 33, 59, 6163, 74, 7678, 81, 86, 90, 92, 93, 96, 98, 104106, 114], memories for public events [10, 80, 81], life scripts [32, 71, 84, 87, 101], and other heterogeneous responses [79, 83, 94, 100].

The exact location of the bump varied with each method for activating memories. For instance, with the important memories method, studies showed the bump between 10–30 years of age [11, 22, 32, 33, 35, 65, 67, 69, 71, 84, 87, 93, 95, 100]. For word cuing method, the bump began as early as 5 years, and lasted until as late as 30 years of age [10, 17, 77, 78, 86, 114]. Likewise, for the studies using life scripts, the location of the bump was from 6 to 39 years. Also, there are a variety of theoretical accounts offering an explanation for the bump for different kinds of memories activated, such as the narrative/identity account which received significant support from eight studies [9, 10, 24, 35, 63, 70, 93, 94], the life story account garnering sound support in four studies [22, 66, 73, 105], and the cultural life script account finding substantial support in two studies [33, 71]. The narrative/identity account received partial support from seven studies [10, 24, 72, 76, 108, 111, 112], and the cultural life script account received a degree of support from ten studies [32, 60, 75, 87, 89, 100103, 114].

Interpretation of study findings

Past research indicates that the cues used to induce the memories influence both the proportion of memories recalled and the location of the bump [8, 15]. A variety of cuing methods exist, therefore different retrieval processes help to explain the differences in reported location of the bump and the distribution of AMs [117]. The research demonstrates that word cues initiate an associative, “bottom-up” process in memory, whereas the important memories method prompts a strategic, “top-down” process in memory that is organized around important memories [8, 12]. The Attention-to-Memory hypothesis [118, 119], proposes that the two major brain regions playing different roles in attention are the dorsal parietal cortex and the ventral parietal cortex. The dorsal parietal cortex is associated with top-down attention (i.e. selecting stimuli on the basis of the internal goals of the individual); the ventral parietal cortex is concerned with bottom-up attention (i.e. permitting the detection of related stimuli) [120, 121].

The Attention-to-Memory hypothesis states that in addition to playing a significant role in attention, the two cortexes play similar roles in memory retrieval [122]. The dorsal parietal cortex initiates the assignment of attentional resources towards retrieval of a specific memory (i.e. top-down Attention-to-Memory) [122]. The important memories method supports this, as the dorsal parietal cortex initiates top-down attention when retrieval relies on memory search [8, 12]. Alternatively, in word cuing methods, the ventral parietal cortex initiates a bottom-up attention focus on the basis of retrieved content. Recent reviews on recognition memory studies support these theories and the localization of the top-down and bottom-up attention [123]. The instructions for the important memories method initiate a search for relevant memories—a role of the dorsal parietal cortex. The instructions for memories related to word cues initiate rapid detection of memory content—a role of the ventral parietal cortex.

Memory activation methods play a significant role in the nature of the memories activated. Word cuing methods yield unbiased sampling of memories across the entire life span [12], whereas the important memories method focuses on eliciting the most important memories of a person’s life, and tends to produce a narrative-based search [8, 12]. Important and self-defining memories are closely linked to the meaning-making processes of individuals [124, 125]. The different types of memory activation methods and theoretical accounts of the bump have common underlying mechanisms influencing bump location. The range and location of the bump vary according to memory activation method: word cuing methods yield a disproportionately large number of recent memories and an earlier bump location (see Table 2), as well as with respect to the type of memories activated. Differing locations of the bump have significant implications for theoretical accounts explaining the bump [8]. In this review, the researchers present a general range of the bump approximated through the analysis of all studies: 16–30 years of age for the important memories method; 5–30 years of age for word cuing methods. A past review paper calculated the mean range and midpoint of the bump formed through different cuing methods. The mean range of bump for word cued and important memories was calculated between 8.7 to 22.5 and 15.1 to 27.9 years of age, respectively [8]. The differences in location of the bump could be due to different methods of activating memories or differences in memory types.

Disparate bump ranges are indicative of the processes occurring at retrieval, favoring a retrieval-based account of the bump. This contradicts the accounts focusing on characteristics of the memories themselves (i.e. narrative/identity account and cognitive account) [11, 24, 25, 35, 40], or the effectiveness of encoding during the bump period (i.e. cognitive abilities account) [11, 126]. These findings suggest a schema-based explanation of the bump (i.e. cultural life script account) rather than an individualistic and memory-based account [32]. There is considerable supporting evidence for the role of cultural life scripts in organizing the retrieval of AMs for important and emotional events [32, 33, 68, 125, 127]. According to Rubin (2015), variations in bump peaks cannot be explained solely in terms of encoding, or by theoretical accounts, especially those that consider adolescence and early adulthood periods to be when the emergence of identity or heightened cognitive ability occurs [3, 11, 117]. Different methods of activating memories give rise to different bumps and the mechanism underlying the bumps for different types of memories are different. The mechanism is different when other kinds of responses or memories are elicited (e.g., life scripts, dreams, etc.)

Life scripts allow encoding and rehearsal of an event by attributing the personal events to some culturally shared importance. A majority of the life script events are anticipated, prepared for, and given a certain meaning before they even happen in a person’s life [32]. According to established empirical evidence, the recall of important life events is structured by the life scripts, however, there is no such structuring of life events through cues, as cues are not likely to initiate culturally shared schemata for important transitional events [46]. The cultural life script account is possibly the reason behind the prevalence of important memories in the bump and a greater proportion of life script events in important memories.

This review demonstrates that the cultural life script account received considerable support, yet accepting it as a possible explanation for the bump would be problematic owing to some inherent flaws. The cultural life script account utilizes the concept of life scripts—cultural expectations about the timing and arrangement of significant transitional life events in a prototypical life course—to provide a cultural explanation for the bump [3133]. The empirical evidence partially supporting the cultural life script account also employed other theoretical accounts to explain these findings [32, 60, 75, 87, 89, 100103, 114]. Very few studies have compared the life script and real life events [68, 75]. Thus, there is a lack of evidence for comparison of the bump patterns for life script events and real life events, and the similarity between the cultural life script events and non-scripted events [33, 60, 68, 128]. The similar bump patterns for both life script events and real life stories, run contrary to the premise of the cultural life script account, meaning that the cultural life script is not solely responsible for the recall of the events and formation of the bump [32].

The fact that the bump is not based on the age of the memories, but on the age of the person recalling at the time of encoding, implies that findings of an artifactual, retrieval based account of the bump (i.e. the cultural life script account) can be rejected [3]. Memories may be easily retrieved due to the originality of the experiences (i.e. high emotional valence), or because they play a role in higher-order structures of the personality (i.e. significance or self-relevance). A bump for vivid memories may occur due to salience rather than mere nostalgia, but are significant because they define who a person is [24]. The ability of a person to consciously recall memories, to identify them as linked to his/her personal past, and to relate them to his/her goals and desires permits the formation of a coherent personal narrative concerned with the present and the future [111, 129].

Researchers propose that there is an emergence of adult identity during late adolescence and early adulthood [130]. This period may contain many self-defining incidents which link the self of an individual to that particular reality [131]. Therefore, an account focusing on the role of self in the bump (e.g. narrative/identity account) can be used to explain the occurrence of memories from this period [62]. The bump reveals an era in an individual’s life that is crucial for the development and maintenance of a stable self, as basic cognitive changes across the life span cannot be solely responsible for shaping retrieval [11, 130]. It is likely that the development of a new self initiates preferential encoding due to the importance of the formation of particular personal and cultural identities [22, 31, 132]. As AMs ground the self, there is a possibility that the importance of identity development stimulates the use of cognitive mechanisms [11, 36].

The narrative/identity account received significant support: a study analyzing dream content, temporally linked with the bump, revealed themes associated with identity and life goals [9]. However, the dreams were collected from professional career women mostly at retirement age, who were already experiencing a transition—during which most people are concerned with life orientation and purpose—that could trigger the importance of identity. Another study collected benchmarked memories from the life history timeline, revealing a bump associated with identity formation in early adulthood, however, the bump was seen concerning only family or relationships [70]. A high correlation between levels of rehearsal and preoccupation with stories from participants’ lives could show potential support for the narrative/identity account, but causal claims cannot yet be made [35].

Free recall of public and private news items revealed differential bumps: 10 to 19 years for public items and 20 to 29 years for private items [42]. The earlier bump reflects a period of formation of generation identity, while the later bump mirrors a period of formation of intimate relationships [42]. The generation of self-images in the form of “I am” statements to test the relationship between memory accessibility and self, lends support to the narrative/identity account due to the clustering of AMs around the time of self-formation [93]. However, this evidence was only found when the first three memories representing each self were compared at age 20 versus age 40. Another study revealed an absence of the bump for AMs when highly self-relevant life events were supressed [69].

Similar support is shown in studies by examining the relationship of highly positive and highly negative events with life story and identity [107]; and the role of generational identity with the development of an integrative self behind the bump [62]. The latter study demonstrates an accessibility of AMs from a period outside of the reminiscence bump that are suggested to be relevant to the self [111]. In this study, a group of older Bangladeshi participants presented a second bump for the ages of 35 to 55 years, coinciding with the period of Bangladesh’s war for independence in 1971. It is suggested that the second bump is due to the enhanced retrieval of AMs from a period when Bengalis, as a nation, were struggling to establish their own independent country, and to uphold their collective Bengali identity. However, a current debate exists on whether individuals recall public events from the bump period because of their identification with those events, or because of better encoding of them [80]. The narrative/identity account states that all the memories for adolescent events are not necessarily self-narrative memories, but rather that more events from this phase, with a better availability for recall, form a pool for self-narrative memories.

Alternatively, the ratings of re-living and vividness showed no differences between bump and non-bump memories, thus rejecting the role of phenomenological features of memories in the bump formation [82]. One study investigating the personal significance of songs showed a bump for both R (remember) and K (know) ratings [94]. Although a greater number of personally significant songs were associated with R ratings, K ratings also formed a bump, even though according to the narrative/identity account, they should not do so. The question of circularity raised in the research has not been answered (i.e. whether the selection of songs as personally significant is due to their association with particular memories, or high personal importance of a song leads a person to relate this song to specific memories from the time it was heard) [94].

The preponderance of memories from the bump period does not mean that these memories are related to identity formation unless there is a direct retrieval of identity related memories and an analysis of the lifespan distribution of these memories is performed. Although the narrative/identity account states that the bump is the result of an identity-relevant process, substantial evidence is still needed. Furthermore, the way identity-related questions are formulated, and the functional demands of answering these questions for the participants, impacts the construction of available memories. Various factors such as emotional salience, specific temporal and geographical context, sociocultural factors and a self-reference effect might influence the preferential retrieval of personally significant, over non-significant, events [133, 134]. As a result of the interplay between these factors, information associated with the self tends to be remembered best [135]; and thus, these more easily remembered memories may not necessarily indicate that bump memories are from important identity-forming events.

The heterogeneous methodologies of the studies in this review have created difficulty for the authors in discerning a link between identity formation and the bump, as the studies are based on memories, or important memories, and ratings. A major issue with ratings lies in the fact that these ratings reflect how study participants currently feel about their experiences rather than what they felt at the time of encoding the memories. Therefore, ratings have a limited role in identity related explanations of the bump (e.g. it is difficult to ascertain how study participants recalled and reported their judgments of rehearsal to reflect true/precise rehearsal rates).

The authors did not find any direct request to recall specific self-defining memories (SDMs) in the research, while important life event narratives were requested. Two studies investigated the role of the self in AMs by examining SDMs [136]. The exploration of SDMs is an important approach for understanding the association between identity and the bump as they are memories of events that one draws on to inform one’s sense of identity [137, 138]. A few studies did investigate the self-relevance of autobiographical events in the bump and their centrality to the life story and identity, SDMs, and self-images [93, 107, 108, 111]. The identity of individuals depends upon their ability to recall personal history, in the form of self-defining memories [139]. Therefore, there is a need to look deeper into the encoding and retrieval of event-specific temporal knowledge for understanding the self and identity [140].

The key to understanding the bump may lie in the memories of self-defining events during adolescence and early adulthood, as the narrative/identity account claims that many memories found in the bump are from this period [24, 93, 131, 141]. These memories play a vital role in the regulation of mood and direct functions of the self [142, 143]. Studies using measures for memory function and the self asked participants to report 20 “I am” self-concepts, thereby collecting concepts/roles significant to their definitions of self [39, 93, 144, 145].

Theoretical implications

The present systematic review extends the body of knowledge on the reminiscence bump, highlights theoretical accounts giving various explanations for the bump, and supports the use of a variety of methods of identity construction as possible explanations for the bump phenomenon. It shows that varying locations of the bump could be due to different methods of activating memories or differences in memory types. Future research could examine memory functions and the measure of the self, along with the role of SDMs in the association of identity and the bump. New research could also be conducted on the salience of identity in memories, and the significance of goals in SDM formation. The role of SDMs in helping familiarize an individual to age-related changes could be investigated.

The authors highlight a gap in the research for which, if any, theoretical account offers the best explanation for the bump. The reviewed studies do not provide enough evidence to construct a clear understanding of the bump and its location and formation using different types of memory assessments. There is a need to conduct further studies investigating methods of memory activation, different types of memories activated, and theories for the bump, particularly to compare the plausibility of several theoretical accounts simultaneously in a single study. A novel research strategy could be developed for use in a large study to determine if the narrative/identity account, or cultural life script account, better explains the bump.

Strengths/Limitations

This review provides a foundation for a more transparent understanding of the relative plausibility of theoretical accounts explaining the bump as reflected in the research. Since the temporal location of the bump varies according to memory activation method, the authors present the bump’s most widely accepted location. This review includes studies in various languages and geographical locations, and with differing population characteristics and lengths. The researchers conducted an extensive quality assessment exercise for study inclusion.

A limitation of this systematic review is simply the heterogeneity of the pool of studies regarding research design, time period conducted, sample size, sample characteristics, intervention strategies implemented during different time periods, and assessment method. Methods used to examine the bump depend on self-report measures, and cannot be evaluated for accuracy of recall due to the potential for self-report bias, which may influence results. The 14 criteria checklist [55] used to assess the quality of studies with diverse designs has inherent limitations. Although it permits a comparison among studies for quality, it gives no guidance for what score is considered “good,” or represents a satisfactory level of internal validity. The authors adopted this checklist based on its use in other published systematic reviews [146, 147].

Conclusion

This systematic review provides a comprehensive summary of the empirical research on the reminiscence bump published between 1988 and 2017. Findings illustrate that the cuing method and important memories method were widely used to induce memories. Results indicate the overall temporal location of the bump to be between 10–30 years of age for the important memories method, 5–30 years of age for word cuing methods, and 6–39 years of age for studies using life scripts. Both the narrative/identity and cultural life scripts accounts received a fair amount of support in explaining the occurrence of the bump. The authors indicate a need for further research in identifying: (a) the theoretical account offering the most comprehensive explanation for the bump, and (b) the most accurate method(s) of memory activation. The strengths and limitations of both accounts of the bump (i.e., the narrative/identity account and cultural life script account) and suggestions for future studies are discussed. The current, empirical evidence on the bump summarized in this paper could be valuable for researchers and professionals in the fields of cognitive psychology and neuroscience.

Supporting information

S1 Table. Showing Key words and alternative words.

Computer-based searches were conducted to search nine databases. In each search, derivatives of “reminiscence bump” were combined using the Boolean OR operator and wildcards.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0208595.s001

(DOCX)

S2 Table. Databases searched for the systematic review.

A search of nine databases gave a total of 523 research articles.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0208595.s002

(DOCX)

S3 Table. Showing quality assessment of quantitative studies included in this systematic review (n = 68).

The detailed quality assessment of all included studies was carried out through a 14 criteria given by Kmet, Lee, and Cook.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0208595.s003

(DOCX)

S4 Table. PRISMA checklist.

A PRISMA checklist showing various section of this review and page numbers on which these sections are reported.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0208595.s004

(DOCX)

S1 Fig. Geographical distribution of all 68 studies examined in this systematic review.

The clustering of studies on the basis of geographical location shows that most of the studies (n = 19) were conducted in USA.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0208595.s005

(TIF)

S2 Fig. Temporal distribution of all 68 studies examined in this systematic review.

The clustering of included studies in three groups; 1988 to 1999; 2000 to 2010, and 2011 to 2017.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0208595.s006

(TIF)

S1 File. PROSPERO protocol.

Review protocol registered in PROSPERO (International prospective register of systematic reviews).

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0208595.s007

(PDF)

Acknowledgments

Dr. Shogo Moriya, Senior Lecturer, BRIMS, Jeffrey Cheah School of Medicine & Health Sciences, Monash University Malaysia for his assistance in reviewing and extracting data from the Japanese research article. Monash University Malaysia is acknowledged for offering a Higher Degree by Research Scholarship to Khadeeja Munawar. The authors also acknowledge Global Asia in the 21st Century (GA21) Platform at Monash University Malaysia for the open access publication funding support.

References

  1. 1. Rubin DC. The distribution of early childhood memories. Memory. 2000;8(4):265–9. pmid:10932795
  2. 2. Rubin DC, Wenzel AE. One hundred years of forgetting: A quantitative description of retention. Psychological review. 1996;103(4):734. 00006832-199610000-00006.
  3. 3. Rubin DC, Wetzler SE, Nebes RD. Autobiographical memory across the lifespan. In: Rubin DC, editor. Autobiographical memory. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press; 1986. p. 202–21.
  4. 4. Scherman AZ. Cultural life script theory and the reminiscence bump: A reanalysis of seven studies across cultures. Nordic Psychology. 2013;65(2):103–19.
  5. 5. Eysenck MW, Keane MT. Cognitive psychology: a student's handbook. East Sussex, England: Psychology Press.; 2010.
  6. 6. Goldstein EB. Cognitive psychology: Connecting mind, research, and everyday experience. 2 ed. Belmont, CA: Thomson Wadsworth.; 2008.
  7. 7. Rathbone C, Moulin A, Conway M, Holmes E. Autobiographical memory and the self. Cognitive psychology. 2012;2:546–76.
  8. 8. Koppel J, Berntsen D. The peaks of life: The differential temporal locations of the reminiscence bump across disparate cueing methods. Journal of Applied Research in Memory and Cognition. 2015;4(1):66–80.
  9. 9. Cappeliez P. An explanation of the reminiscence bump in the dreams of older adults in terms of life goals and identity. Self and Identity. 2008;7(1):25–33.
  10. 10. Holmes A, Conway MA. Generation identity and the reminiscence bump: Memory for public and private events. Journal of Adult Development. 1999;6(1):21–34.
  11. 11. Rubin DC, Rahhal TA, Poon LW. Things learned in early adulthood are remembered best. Memory & cognition. 1998;26(1):3–19.
  12. 12. Crovitz HF, Schiffman H. Frequency of episodic memories as a function of their age. Bulletin of the Psychonomic Society. 1974;4(5):517–8.
  13. 13. Galton F. Psychometric experiments. Brain. 1879;2(2):149–62.
  14. 14. Janssen SMJ, Rubin DC, St. Jacques PL. The temporal distribution of autobiographical memory: changes in reliving and vividness over the life span do not explain the reminiscence bump. Memory & Cognition. 2011;39(1):1–11. pmid:21264610
  15. 15. Rubin DC, Schulkind MD. Distribution of important and word-cued autobiographical memories in 20-, 35-, and 70-year-old adults. Psychology and aging. 1997;12:524–35. pmid:9308099
  16. 16. Rubin DC, Schulkind MD. The distribution of autobiographical memories across the lifespan. Memory & cognition. 1997;25(6):859–66.
  17. 17. Schuman H, Corning A. Collective memory and autobiographical memory: Similar but not the same. Memory Studies. 2014;7(2):146–60.
  18. 18. Fitzgerald JM, Lawrence R. Autobiographical memory across the life-span. Journal of gerontology. 1984;39(6):692–8. Epub 1984/11/01. pmid:6491180.
  19. 19. Franklin HC, Holding DH. Personal memories at different ages. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology. 1977;29(3):527–32.
  20. 20. Zola-Morgan S, Cohen NJ, Squire LR. Recall of remote episodic memory in amnesia. Neuropsychologia. 1983;21(5):487–500. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/0028-3932(83)90005-2. pmid:6646401
  21. 21. Cuervo-Lombard C, Jovenin N, Hedelin GUY, Rizzo-Peter L, Conway MA, Danion J-M. Autobiographical memory of adolescence and early adulthood events: An investigation in schizophrenia. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society. 2007;13(2):335–43. Epub 02/02. pmid:17286890
  22. 22. Glück J, Bluck S. Looking back across the life span: A life story account of the reminiscence bump. Memory & cognition. 2007;35(8):1928–39.
  23. 23. Benson KA, Jarvi SD, Arai Y, Thielbar PR, Frye KJ, McDonald BLG. Socio-historical context and autobiographical memories: Variations in the reminiscence phenomenon. Theoretical perspectives on autobiographical memory: Springer; 1992. p. 313–22.
  24. 24. Fitzgerald JM. Vivid memories and the reminiscence phenomenon: The role of a self narrative. Human Development. 1988;31(5):261–73.
  25. 25. Robinson JA, Taylor LR. Autobiographical memory and self-narratives: A tale of two stories. 1998.
  26. 26. Koppel J. The reminiscence bump for public events: A review of its prevalence and taxonomy of alternative age distributions. Applied Cognitive Psychology. 2013;27(1):12–32.
  27. 27. Schuman H, Rodgers WL. Cohorts, chronology, and collective memories. Public Opinion Quarterly. 2004;68(2):217–54.
  28. 28. Schuman H, Scott J. Generations and collective memories. American sociological review. 1989;54(3):359–81.
  29. 29. Pillemer DB. Momentous events and the life story. Review of General Psychology. 2001;5(2):123.
  30. 30. Janssen SM, Murre JM. Reminiscence bump in autobiographical memory: Unexplained by novelty, emotionality, valence, or importance of personal events. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology. 2008;61(12):1847–60. pmid:19031155
  31. 31. Berntsen D, Rubin DC. Emotionally charged autobiographical memories across the life span: the recall of happy, sad, traumatic, and involuntary memories. Psychology and aging. 2002;17(4):636–52. Epub 2003/01/01. pmid:12507360.
  32. 32. Berntsen D, Rubin DC. Cultural life scripts structure recall from autobiographical memory. Memory & cognition. 2004;32(3):427–42.
  33. 33. Rubin DC, Berntsen D. Life scripts help to maintain autobiographical memories of highly positive, but not highly negative, events. Memory & cognition. 2003;31(1):1–14.
  34. 34. Conway MA, Holmes A. Psychosocial stages and the accessibility of autobiographical memories across the life cycle. Journal of personality. 2004;72(3):461–80. Epub 2004/04/23. pmid:15102035.
  35. 35. Fitzgerald JM. The distribution of self-narrative memories in younger and older adults: Elaborating the self-narrative hypothesis. Aging, Neuropsychology, and Cognition. 1996;3(3):229–36.
  36. 36. Conway MA. Memory and the self. Journal of Memory and Language. 2005;53(4):594–628. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jml.2005.08.005.
  37. 37. Kormi-Nouri R, Nilsson LG, Ohta N. The novelty effect: support for the Novelty-Encoding Hypothesis. Scandinavian journal of psychology. 2005;46(2):133–43. Epub 2005/03/15. pmid:15762941.
  38. 38. Piefke M, Fink GR. Recollections of one’s own past: the effects of aging and gender on the neural mechanisms of episodic autobiographical memory. Anatomy and Embryology. 2005;210(5):497–512. pmid:16172875
  39. 39. Rose Addis D, Tippett L. Memory of myself: Autobiographical memory and identity in Alzheimer's disease. Memory. 2004;12(1):56–74. pmid:15098621
  40. 40. Conway MA, Pleydell-Pearce CW. The construction of autobiographical memories in the self-memory system. Psychol Rev2000. p. 261–88. pmid:10789197
  41. 41. McAdams DP. Identity and the life story. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates Publishers; 2003.
  42. 42. Power MJ. The multistory self: Why the self is more than the sum of its autoparts. Journal of Clinical Psychology. 2007;63(2):187–98. pmid:17173323
  43. 43. Ricarte JJ, Ros L, Latorre JM, Watkins E. Mapping autobiographical memory in schizophrenia: Clinical implications. Clinical Psychology Review. 2017;51(Supplement C):96–108. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cpr.2016.11.004.
  44. 44. Schank RC. Dynamic memory revisited. New York: Cambridge University Press; 1999.
  45. 45. Schank R C, P AR, editors. Scripts, plan, and knowledge. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press; 1977.
  46. 46. Rubin DC, Berntsen D, Hutson M. The Normative and the Personal Life: Individual Differences in Life Scripts and Life Story Events among U.S.A. and Danish Undergraduates. Memory (Hove, England). 2009;17(1):54–68. PMC3042895. pmid:19105087
  47. 47. Janssen SMJ, Haque S. The transmission and stability of cultural life scripts: a cross-cultural study. Memory. 2018;26(1):131–43. pmid:28585471
  48. 48. Neugarten BL, Moore JW, Lowe JC. AGE NORMS, AGE CONSTRAINTS, AND ADULT SOCIALIZATION. AJS; American journal of sociology. 1965;70:710–7. Epub 1965/05/01. pmid:14298073.
  49. 49. Shai D. Working Women/Cloistered Men: A Family Development Approach to Marriage Arrangements among Ultra-orthodox Jews. Journal of Comparative Family Studies. 2002;33(1):97–115.
  50. 50. Clarke J. What is a systematic review? Evidence Based Nursing. 2011;14(3):64. pmid:21659560
  51. 51. Uman LS. Systematic Reviews and Meta-Analyses. Journal of the Canadian Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry. 2011;20(1):57–9. PMC3024725. pmid:21286370
  52. 52. Moher D, Liberati A, Tetzlaff J, Altman DG, Group P. Preferred reporting items for systematic reviews and meta-analyses: the PRISMA statement. PLoS med. 2009;6(7):e1000097. pmid:19621072
  53. 53. Munawar K, Haque S. The formation and understanding of the reminiscence bump: A systematic review 2017. Available from: http://www.crd.york.ac.uk/PROSPERO/display_record.php?ID=CRD42017076695.
  54. 54. Dinet J, Favart M, Passerault J-M. Searching for information in an online public access catalogue (OPAC): the impacts of information search expertise on the use of Boolean operators. Journal of Computer Assisted Learning. 2004;20(5):338–46.
  55. 55. Kmet LM, Lee RC, Cook LS. Standard quality assessment criteria for evaluating primary research papers from a variety of fields. Alberta Heritage Foundation for Medical Research Edmonton; 2004.
  56. 56. Wassenaar A, Schouten J, Schoonhoven L. Factors promoting intensive care patients’ perception of feeling safe: A systematic review. International journal of nursing studies. 2014;51(2):261–73. pmid:23910399
  57. 57. Stierlin AS, De Lepeleere S, Cardon G, Dargent-Molina P, Hoffmann B, Murphy MH, et al. A systematic review of determinants of sedentary behaviour in youth: a DEDIPAC-study. International Journal of Behavioral Nutrition and Physical Activity. 2015;12(1):133.
  58. 58. Dempster M. A research guide for health and clinical psychology. Basingstoke: Palgrave Macmillan; 2011. https://doi.org/10.1002/jclp.20803
  59. 59. Alea N, Ali S, Marcano B. The bumps in Trinidadian life: Reminiscence bumps for positive and negative life events. Applied Cognitive Psychology. 2014;28(2):174–84.
  60. 60. Bohn A, Berntsen D. The reminiscence bump reconsidered: Children’s prospective life stories show a bump in young adulthood. Psychological Science. 2011;22(2):197–202. pmid:21193779
  61. 61. Chu S, Downes JJ. Long live Proust: The odour-cued autobiographical memory bump. Cognition. 2000;75(2):B41–B50. pmid:10771279
  62. 62. Conway MA, Haque S. Overshadowing the reminiscence bump: Memories of a struggle for independence. Journal of Adult Development. 1999;6(1):35–44.
  63. 63. Conway MA, Wang Q, Hanyu K, Haque S. A cross-cultural investigation of autobiographical memory on the universality and cultural variation of the reminiscence bump. Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology. 2005;36(6):739–49.
  64. 64. Copeland DE, Radvansky GA, Goodwin KA. A novel study: Forgetting curves and the reminiscence bump. Memory. 2009;17(3):323–36. pmid:19206017
  65. 65. Davison IM, Feeney A. Regret as autobiographical memory. Cognitive psychology. 2008;57(4):385–403. pmid:18455156
  66. 66. Demiray B, Gülgöz S, Bluck S. Examining the life story account of the reminiscence bump: Why we remember more from young adulthood. Memory. 2009;17(7):708–23. pmid:19598057
  67. 67. Denver JY, Lane SM, Cherry KE. Recent versus remote: Flashbulb memory for 9/11 and self-selected events from the reminiscence bump. The International Journal of Aging and Human Development. 2010;70(4):275–97. pmid:20649160
  68. 68. Dickson RA, Pillemer DB, Bruehl EC. The reminiscence bump for salient personal memories: Is a cultural life script required? Memory & cognition. 2011;39(6):977. pmid:21424878
  69. 69. Ece B, Gülgöz S. The impact of suppressing the typical life events on the reminiscence bump. Applied Cognitive Psychology. 2014;28(5):702–10.
  70. 70. Elnick AB, Margrett JA, Fitzgerald JM, Labouvie-Vief G. Benchmark Memories in Adulthood: Central Domains and Predictors of Their Frequency. Journal of Adult Development. 1999;6(1):45–59.
  71. 71. Erdoğan A, Baran B, Avlar B, Taş AÇ, Tekcan AI. On the persistence of positive events in life scripts. Applied Cognitive Psychology. 2008;22(1):95–111.
  72. 72. Fromholt P, Larsen SF. Autobiographical memory in normal aging and primary degenerative dementia (dementia of Alzheimer type). Journal of Gerontology. 1991;46(3):P85–P91. https://doi.org/10.1093/geronj/46.3.P85. pmid:2030279
  73. 73. Fromholt P, Mortensen D, Torpdahl P, Bender L, Larsen P, Rubin D. Life-narrative and word-cued autobiographical memories in centenarians: comparisons with 80-year-old control, depressed, and dementia groups. Memory. 2003;11(1):81–8. pmid:12653490
  74. 74. Gidron Y, Alon S. Autobiographical memory and depression in the later age: The bump is a turning point. The International Journal of Aging and Human Development. 2007;64(1):1–11. pmid:17390962
  75. 75. Haque S, Hasking PA. Life scripts for emotionally charged autobiographical memories: A cultural explanation of the reminiscence bump. Memory. 2010;18(7):712–29. pmid:20803371
  76. 76. Jansari A, Parkin AJ. Things that go bump in your life: Explaining the reminiscence bump in autobiographical memory. Psychology and aging. 1996;11(1):85. pmid:8726374
  77. 77. Janssen S, Chessa A, Murre J. Modeling the reminiscence bump in autobiographical memory with the Memory Chain Model. Constructive memory. 2003:138–47.
  78. 78. Janssen S, Chessa A, Murre J. The reminiscence bump in autobiographical memory: Effects of age, gender, education, and culture. Memory. 2005;13(6):658–68. pmid:16076679
  79. 79. Janssen SM, Chessa AG, Murre JM. Temporal distribution of favourite books, movies, and records: Differential encoding and re-sampling. Memory. 2007;15(7):755–67. pmid:17852723
  80. 80. Janssen SM, Murre JM, Meeter M. Reminiscence bump in memory for public events. European Journal of Cognitive Psychology. 2008;20(4):738–64.
  81. 81. Janssen SM, Gralak A, Murre JM. A model for removing the increased recall of recent events from the temporal distribution of autobiographical memory. Behavior research methods. 2011;43(4):916–30. pmid:21614661
  82. 82. Janssen SM, Rubin DC, Jacques PLS. The temporal distribution of autobiographical memory: changes in reliving and vividness over the life span do not explain the reminiscence bump. Memory & cognition. 2011;39(1):1–11. pmid:21264610
  83. 83. Janssen SM, Rubin DC, Conway MA. The reminiscence bump in the temporal distribution of the best football players of all time: Pelé, Cruijff or Maradona? The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology. 2012;65(1):165–78. pmid:21939366
  84. 84. Janssen SM. Is There a Cultural Life Script for Public Events? Applied Cognitive Psychology. 2015;29(1):61–8.
  85. 85. Ju I, Choi Y, Morris J, Liao HW, Bluck S. Creating Nostalgic Advertising Based on the Reminiscence Bump: Diachronic Relevance and Purchase Intent. Applied Cognitive Psychology. 2016;30:465–71.
  86. 86. Kawasaki Y, Janssen SM, Inoue T. Temporal distribution of autobiographical memory: Uncovering the reminiscence bump in Japanese young and middle‐aged adults1. Japanese Psychological Research. 2011;53(1):86–96.
  87. 87. Koppel J, Berntsen D. Does everything happen when you are young? Introducing the youth bias. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology. 2014;67(3):417–23. pmid:24286365
  88. 88. Koppel J, Berntsen D. The reminiscence bump in autobiographical memory and for public events: A comparison across different cueing methods. Memory. 2016;24(1):44–62. pmid:25529327
  89. 89. Koppel J, Berntsen D. The reminiscence bump without memories: The distribution of imagined word-cued and important autobiographical memories in a hypothetical 70-year-old. Consciousness and Cognition. 2016;44:89–102. pmid:27376837
  90. 90. Krumhansl CL, Zupnick JA. Cascading reminiscence bumps in popular music. Psychological science. 2013;24(10):2057–68. pmid:24006129
  91. 91. Leist AK, Ferring D, Filipp S-H. Remembering positive and negative life events: Associations with future time perspective and functions of autobiographical memory. GeroPsych: The Journal of Gerontopsychology and Geriatric Psychiatry. 2010;23(3):137.
  92. 92. Platz F, Kopiez R, Hasselhorn J, Wolf A. The impact of song-specific age and affective qualities of popular songs on music-evoked autobiographical memories (MEAMs). Musicae Scientiae. 2015;19(4):327–49.
  93. 93. Rathbone CJ, Moulin CJ, Conway MA. Self-centered memories: The reminiscence bump and the self. Memory & cognition. 2008;36(8):1403–14. pmid:19015500
  94. 94. Rathbone CJ, O’Connor AR, Moulin CJA. The tracks of my years: Personal significance contributes to the reminiscence bump. Memory & cognition. 2017;45(1):137–50. pmid:27566486
  95. 95. Rybash JM, Monaghan BE. Episodic and semantic contributions to older adults' autobiographical recall. The Journal of general psychology. 1999;126(1):85–96. pmid:10216971
  96. 96. Schrauf RW, Rubin DC. Bilingual autobiographical memory in older adult immigrants: A test of cognitive explanations of the reminiscence bump and the linguistic encoding of memories. Journal of Memory and Language. 1998;39(3):437–57.
  97. 97. Schrauf RW, Rubin DC. Effects of voluntary immigration on the distribution of autobiographical memory over the lifespan. Applied Cognitive Psychology. 2001;15(7).
  98. 98. Schubert E. Does recall of a past music event invoke a reminiscence bump in young adults? Memory. 2016;24(7):1007–14. pmid:26275069
  99. 99. Steiner KL, Pillemer DB, Thomsen DK, Minigan AP. The reminiscence bump in older adults' life story transitions. Memory. 2014;22(8):1002–9. pmid:24295374
  100. 100. Svob C, Brown NR. Intergenerational transmission of the reminiscence bump and biographical conflict knowledge. Psychological Science. 2012;23(11):1404–9. pmid:23012267
  101. 101. Tekcan Aİ, Kaya-Kızılöz B, Odaman H. Life scripts across age groups: A comparison of adolescents, young adults, and older adults. Memory. 2012;20(8):836–47. pmid:22897157
  102. 102. Thomsen DK, Berntsen D. The cultural life script and life story chapters contribute to the reminiscence bump. Memory. 2008;16(4):420–35. pmid:18432486
  103. 103. Thomsen DK, Pillemer DB, Ivcevic Z. Life story chapters, specific memories and the reminiscence bump. Memory. 2011;19(3):267–79. pmid:21500087
  104. 104. Webster JD, Gould O. Reminiscence and vivid personal memories across adulthood. The International Journal of Aging and Human Development. 2007;64(2):149–70. pmid:17451043
  105. 105. Wolf T, Zimprich D. How can individual differences in autobiographical memory distributions of older adults be explained? Memory. 2016;24(9):1287–99. pmid:26492840
  106. 106. Maki Y, Naka M. Reminiscence bump and contents of autobiographical memory in Japanese elders. Shinrigaku kenkyu: The Japanese journal of psychology. 2006;77(4):333–41. pmid:17447443
  107. 107. Berntsen D, Rubin DC, Siegler I. Two Versions of Life: Emotionally Negative and Positive Life Events Have Different Roles in The Organization of Life Story and Identity. Emotion. 2011;11(5):1190–201. pmid:21875191
  108. 108. Raffard S, D’Argembeau A, Lardi C, Bayard S, Boulenger J-P, Linden MVd. Narrative identity in schizophrenia. Consciousness and Cognition. 2010;19(1):328–40. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.concog.2009.10.005. pmid:19955004
  109. 109. Schlagman S, Kliegel M, Schulz J, Kvavilashvili L. Differential Effects of Age on Involuntary and Voluntary Autobiographical Memory. Psychol Aging. 2009;24(2):397–411. pmid:19485657
  110. 110. Schroots JJF, Dijkum CV, Assink MHJ. Autobiographical Memory from a Life Span Perspective. The International Journal of Aging and Human Development. 2004;58(1):69–85. pmid:15248473.
  111. 111. Raffard S, D'Argembeau A, Lardi C, Bayard S, Boulenger J-P, Boulenger J-P, et al. Exploring self-defining memories in schizophrenia. Memory. 2009;17(1):26–38. pmid:19105085
  112. 112. Cuervo-lombard C, Jovenin N, Hedelin G, Rizzo-peter L, Conway MA, Danion J-m. Autobiographical memory of adolescence and early adulthood events: An investigation in schizophrenia. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society: JINS. 2007;13(2):335–43. pmid:17286890; 17286890.
  113. 113. Berntsen D, Rubin D. Emotionally charged autobiographical memories across the life span: The recall of happy, sad, traumatic, and inventory memories. Psychology and aging. 2002;17(4):636–52. pmid:12507360
  114. 114. Maki Y, Janssen SMJ, Uemiya A, Naka M. The phenomenology and temporal distributions of autobiographical memories elicited with emotional and neutral cue words. Memory. 2013;21(3):286–300. pmid:23215874
  115. 115. Popay J, Roberts H, Sowden A, Petticrew M, Arai L, Rodgers M, et al. Guidance on the conduct of narrative synthesis in systematic reviews. A product from the ESRC methods programme Version. 2006;1:b92.
  116. 116. Rybash JM. Aging and autobiographical memory: The long and bumpy road. Journal of Adult Development. 1999;6(1):1–10.
  117. 117. Rubin DC. One bump, two bumps, three bumps, four? Using retrieval cues to divide one autobiographical memory reminiscence bump into many. Journal of Applied Research in Memory and Cognition. 2015;4(1):87–9.
  118. 118. Cabeza R. Role of parietal regions in episodic memory retrieval: The dual attentional processes hypothesis. Neuropsychologia. 2008;46(7):1813–27. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2008.03.019. pmid:18439631
  119. 119. Ciaramelli E, Grady CL, Moscovitch M. Top-down and bottom-up attention to memory: A hypothesis (AtoM) on the role of the posterior parietal cortex in memory retrieval. Neuropsychologia. 2008;46(7):1828–51. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2008.03.022. pmid:18471837
  120. 120. Corbetta M, Shulman G. Control of goal-directed and stimulus-driven attention in the brain. Nat Rev Neurosci2002. p. 201–15. pmid:11994752
  121. 121. Marois R, Leung H-C, Gore JC. A Stimulus-Driven Approach to Object Identity and Location Processing in the Human Brain. Neuron. 2000;25(3):717–28. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0896-6273(00)81073-9. pmid:10774738
  122. 122. Ciaramelli E, Grady C, Levine B, Ween J, Moscovitch M. Top-down and bottom-up attention to memory are dissociated in posterior parietal cortex: neuroimagingand and neuropsychological evidence. The Journal of neuroscience: the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience. 2010;30(14):4943. pmid:20371815
  123. 123. Hutchinson J, Uncapher M, Wagner AD. Posterior parietal cortex and episodic retrieval: Convergent and divergent effects of attention and memory. Learn Mem2009. p. 343–56.
  124. 124. Hsiao-Wen L, Susan B, Gerben JW. Longitudinal Relations Between Self-Defining Memories and Self-Esteem: Mediating Roles of Meaning-Making and Memory Function. Imagination, Cognition and Personality. 2017:0276236617733840.
  125. 125. Berntsen D, Bohn A. Remembering and forecasting: The relation. Memory & cognition. 2010;38(3):265–78. pmid:20234017
  126. 126. Janssen SMJ, Kristo G, Rouw R, Murre JMJ. The relation between verbal and visuospatial memory and autobiographical memory. Consciousness and Cognition. 2015;31(Supplement C):12–23. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.concog.2014.10.001.
  127. 127. Bohn A. Generational differences in cultural life scripts and life story memories of younger and older adults. Applied Cognitive Psychology. 2010;24(9):1324–45.
  128. 128. Radvansky GA. Human Memory: Third Edition. 3 ed. New York: Taylor & Francis; 2017. 674 p.
  129. 129. Gallagher S. Philosophical conceptions of the self: implications for cognitive science. Trends in Cognitive Sciences. 2000;4(1):14–21. https://doi.org/10.1016/S1364-6613(99)01417-5. pmid:10637618
  130. 130. Erikson EH. Childhood and society. New York: W.W. Norton; 1950.
  131. 131. Singer JA, Salovey P. The remembered self: emotion and memory in personality. Salovey P, editor. New York: Toronto: Maxwell Macmillan International; 1993.
  132. 132. Hockey R. Rate of presentation in running memory and direct manipulation of input-processing strategies. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology. 1973;25(1):104–11.
  133. 133. Westmacott R, Moscovitch M. The contribution of autobiographical significance to semantic memory. Mem Cogn. 2003;31(5):761–74.
  134. 134. On the nature of autobiographical memory. In: Berntsen D, Rubin DC, editors. Understanding autobiographical memory: Theories and approaches New York, NY: Cambridge University Press; 2012. p. 54–69.
  135. 135. Rogers TB, Kuiper NA, Kirker WS. Self-reference and the encoding of personal information. Journal of personality and social psychology. 1977;35(9):677. pmid:909043
  136. 136. Singer JA, Moffitt KH. An Experimental Investigation of Specificity and Generality in Memory Narratives. Imagination, Cognition and Personality. 1992;11(3):233–57.
  137. 137. Singer JA. Memories that matter: How to use self-defining memories to understand and change your life. Oakland: New Harbinger Publications; 2005.
  138. 138. Blagov PS, Singer JA. Four Dimensions of Self-Defining Memories (Specificity, Meaning, Content, and Affect) and Their Relationships to Self-Restraint, Distress, and Repressive Defensiveness. Journal of personality. 2004;72(3):481–511. pmid:15102036
  139. 139. Moffitt KH, Singer JA. Continulty in the Life Story: Self-Defining Memories, Affect, and Approach/Avoidance Personal Strivings. Journal of personality. 1994;62(1):21–43.
  140. 140. Skowronski JJ, Ritchie TD, Walker WR, Betz AL, Sedikides C, Bethencourt LA, et al. Ordering our world: the quest for traces of temporal organization in autobiographical memory.(Report). Journal of Experimental Social Psychology. 2007;43(5):850–6.
  141. 141. Conway MA, Singer J, Tagini A. The self and autobiographical memory: Correspondence and coherence. Soc Cogn2004. p. 491–529.
  142. 142. Pillemer D. Directive functions of autobiographical memory: The guiding power of the specific episode. Memory. 2003;11(2):193–202. pmid:12820831
  143. 143. Bluck S, Glück J. Making Things Better and Learning a Lesson: Experiencing Wisdom Across the Lifespan. Journal of personality. 2004;72(3):543–72. pmid:15102038
  144. 144. Kuhn MH, McPartland TS. An Empirical Investigation of Self-Attitudes. American Sociological Review. 1954;19(1):68–76.
  145. 145. Madson L, Trafimow D. Gender Comparisons in the Private, Collective, and Allocentric Selves. The Journal of Social Psychology. 2001;141(4):551–9. pmid:11577852
  146. 146. Czekierda K, Banik A, Park CL, Luszczynska A. Meaning in life and physical health: systematic review and meta-analysis. Health Psychology Review. 2017;11(4):387–418. pmid:28488471
  147. 147. Gabriel MG, Wakefield CE, Vetsch J, Karpelowsky JS, Darlington A-SE, Grant DM, et al. The Psychosocial Experiences and Needs of Children Undergoing Surgery and Their Parents: A Systematic Review. Journal of Pediatric Health Care. 2017. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.pedhc.2017.08.003.