Skip to main content
Advertisement
Browse Subject Areas
?

Click through the PLOS taxonomy to find articles in your field.

For more information about PLOS Subject Areas, click here.

  • Loading metrics

Cultural transmission modes of music sampling traditions remain stable despite delocalization in the digital age

  • Mason Youngblood

    Roles Conceptualization, Data curation, Formal analysis, Investigation, Methodology, Resources, Software, Validation, Visualization, Writing – original draft, Writing – review & editing

    myoungblood@gradcenter.cuny.edu

    Affiliations Department of Psychology, The Graduate Center, City University of New York, New York, NY, United States of America, Department of Biology, Queens College, City University of New York, Flushing, NY, United States of America

Abstract

Music sampling is a common practice among hip-hop and electronic producers that has played a critical role in the development of particular subgenres. Artists preferentially sample drum breaks, and previous studies have suggested that these may be culturally transmitted. With the advent of digital sampling technologies and social media the modes of cultural transmission may have shifted, and music communities may have become decoupled from geography. The aim of the current study was to determine whether drum breaks are culturally transmitted through musical collaboration networks, and to identify the factors driving the evolution of these networks. Using network-based diffusion analysis we found strong evidence for the cultural transmission of drum breaks via collaboration between artists, and identified several demographic variables that bias transmission. Additionally, using network evolution methods we found evidence that the structure of the collaboration network is no longer biased by geographic proximity after the year 2000, and that gender disparity has relaxed over the same period. Despite the delocalization of communities by the internet, collaboration remains a key transmission mode of music sampling traditions. The results of this study provide valuable insight into how demographic biases shape cultural transmission in complex networks, and how the evolution of these networks has shifted in the digital age.

Introduction

Music sampling, or the use of previously-recorded material in a new composition, is a nearly ubiquitous practice among hip-hop and electronic producers. The usage of drum breaks, or percussion-heavy sequences, ripped from soul and funk records has played a particularly critical role in the development of certain subgenres. For example, “Amen, Brother”, released by The Winstons in 1969, is widely regarded as the most sampled song of all time. Its iconic 4-bar drum break has been described as “genre-constitutive” [1], and can be prominently heard in classic hip-hop and jungle releases by N.W.A and Shy FX [2]. Due to the consistent usage of drum breaks in particular music communities and subgenres [15] some scholars have suggested that they may be culturally transmitted [6], which could occur as a direct result of collaboration between artists or as an indirect effect of community membership.

Before the digital age, artists may have depended upon collaborators for access to the physical source materials and expensive hardware required for sampling [7]. In the 1990s, new technologies like compressed digital audio formats and digital audio workstations made sampling more accessible to a broader audience [8]. Furthermore, the widespread availability of the internet and social media have delocalized communities [9], and allowed global music “scenes” to form around shared interests beyond peer-to-peer file sharing [10, 11]. Individuals in online music communities now have access to the collective knowledge of other members [12, 13], and there is evidence that online communities play a key role in music discovery [14]. Although musicians remain concentrated in historically important music cities (i.e. New York City and Los Angeles in the United States) [15, 16], online music communities also make it possible for artists to establish collaborative relationships independently of geographic location [17]. If more accessible sampling technologies and access to collective knowledge have allowed artists to discover sample sources independently of collaboration [18], then the strength of cultural transmission via collaboration may have decreased over the last couple of decades. Similarly, if online music communities have created opportunities for interactions between potential collaborators, then geographic proximity may no longer structure musical collaboration networks.

Studies of the cultural evolution of music have primarily investigated diversity in musical performances [19] and traditions [20], macro-scale patterns and selective pressures in musical evolution [2124], and the structure and evolution of consumer networks [14, 25, 26]. Although several diffusion chain experiments have addressed how cognitive biases shape musical traits during transmission [2729], few studies have investigated the mechanisms of cultural transmission at the population level [30, 31]. The practice of sampling drum breaks in hip-hop and electronic music is an ideal research model for cultural transmission because of (1) the remarkably high copy fidelity of sampled material, (2) the reliable documentation of sampling events, and (3) the availability of high-resolution collaboration and demographic data for the artists involved. Exhaustive online datasets of sample usage and collaboration make it possible to reconstruct networks of artists and track the diffusion of particular drum breaks from the early 1980s to today. Furthermore, the technological changes that have occurred over the same time period provide a natural experiment for how the digital age has impacted cultural transmission more broadly [32].

The aim of the current study was to determine whether drum breaks are culturally transmitted through musical collaboration networks, and to identify the factors driving the evolution of these networks. We hypothesized that (1) drum breaks are culturally transmitted through musical collaboration networks, and that (2) the strength of cultural transmission via collaboration would decrease after the year 2000. For clarification, the alternative to the first hypothesis is cultural transmission occurring outside of collaborative relationships (i.e. independent sample discovery via “crate-digging” in record stores or online). Previous studies have investigated similar questions using diffusion curve analysis [30], but the validity of inferring transmission mechanisms from cumulative acquisition data has been called into question [33]. Instead, we applied network-based diffusion analysis (NBDA), a recently developed statistical method for determining whether network structure biases the emergence of a novel behavior in a population [34]. As NBDA is most useful in identifying social learning, an ability that is assumed to be present in humans, it has been primarily applied to non-human animal models such as birds, whales, and primates [3537], but the ability to incorporate individual-level variables to nodes makes it uniquely suited to determining what factors bias diffusion more generally. Additionally, we hypothesized that (3) collaboration probability would be decoupled from geographic proximity after the year 2000. To investigate this we applied separable temporal exponential random graph modeling (STERGM), a dynamic extension of ERGM for determining the variables that bias network evolution [38].

Methods

All data used in the current study were collected in September of 2018, in compliance with the terms and conditions of each database. For the primary analysis, the three most heavily sampled drum breaks of all time, “Amen, Brother” by The Winstons, “Think (About It)” by Lyn Collins, and “Funky Drummer” by James Brown, were identified using WhoSampled (https://www.whosampled.com/). The release year and credits for each song listed as having sampled each break were collected using data scraping. In order to avoid name disambiguation, only artists, producers, and remixers with active Discogs links and associated IDs were included in the dataset. In order to investigate potential shifts in transmission strength around 2000, the same method was used to collect data for the eight songs in the “Most Sampled Tracks” on WhoSampled that were released after 1990 (see S1 Appendix). One of these, “I’m Good” by YG, was excluded from the analysis because the sample is primarily used by a single artist. Each set of sampling events collected from WhoSampled was treated as a separate diffusion. All analyses were conducted in R (v 3.3.3).

Collaboration data were retrieved from Discogs (https://www.discogs.com/), a crowdsourced database of music releases. All collaborative releases in the database were extracted and converted to a master list of pairwise collaborations. For each diffusion, pairwise collaborations including two artists in the dataset were used to construct collaboration networks, in which nodes correspond to artists and weighted links correspond to collaboration number. Although some indirect connections between artists were missing from these subnetworks, conducting the analysis with the full dataset was computationally prohibitive and incomplete networks have been routinely used for NBDA in the past [35, 36, 39].

Individual-level variables for artists included in each collaboration network were collected from MusicBrainz (https://musicbrainz.org/), a crowdsourced database with more complete artist information than Discogs, and Spotify (https://www.spotify.com/), one of the most popular music streaming services. Gender and geographic location were retrieved from the Musicbrainz API. Whenever it was available, the “begin area” of the artist, or the city in which they began their career, was used instead of their “area”, or country of affiliation, to maximize geographic resolution. Longitudes and latitudes for each location, retrieved using the Data Science Toolkit and Google Maps, were used to calculate each artist’s mean geographic distance from other individuals. Albunack (http://www.albunack.net/), an online tool which draws from both Musicbrainz and Discogs, was used to convert IDs between the two databases. Popularity and followers were retrieved using the Spotify API. An artist’s popularity, a proprietary index of streaming count that ranges between 0 and 100, is a better indicator of their long-term success because it is calculated across their entire discography. Followers is a better indicator of current success because it reflects user engagement with artists who are currently more active on the platform. Discogs IDs are incompatible with the Spotify API, so artist names were URL-encoded and used as text search terms.

In order to identify whether social transmission between collaborators played a role in sample acquisition, order of acquisition diffusion analysis (OADA) was conducted using the R script for NBDA (v 1.2.13) provided on the Laland lab’s website (https://lalandlab.st-andrews.ac.uk/freeware/). OADA uses the order in which individuals acquire a trait to determine whether its diffusion is biased by the structure of the social network [40]. OADA was utilized instead of time of acquisition diffusion analysis (TADA) because it makes no assumptions about the baseline rate of acquisition [34]. For each artist, order of acquisition was determined by the year that they first used the sample in their music. Sampling events from the same year were given the same order. Gender, popularity, followers, and mean distance were included as predictor variables. For gender, females were coded as -1, males were coded as 1, and individuals with other identities or missing values were coded as 0. For popularity, followers, and mean distance each variable was centered around zero. Asocial, additive, and multiplicative models were fit to all three diffusions collectively with every possible combination of individual-level variables. Standard information theoretic approaches were used to rank the models according to Akaike’s Information Criterion corrected for sample size (AICc). Models with a ΔAICc < 2 were considered to have the best fit [41]. The best fitting model with the most individual-level variables was run separately to assess the effects of each variable on social transmission. Effect sizes were calculated according to [36]. An additional OADA was conducted using the seven diffusions from after 1990. Individual-level variables were excluded due to insufficient demographic data. An additive model was fit to the OADA, and separate social transmission parameters were calculated for each diffusion to identify differences in transmission strength. Additive and multiplicative models give identical results in the absence of individual-level variables, so no model comparison was necessary.

In order to assess the effects of individual-level variables on network evolution, STERGM was conducted using statnet (v 2016.9), an R package for network analysis and simulation. STERGM is a dynamic social network method that models the formation and dissolution of links over time [38]. Collaboration events involving artists from each diffusion were combined to construct static collaboration subnetworks for each year between 1984 and 2017, which were then converted into an undirected, unweighted dynamic network. Early years not continuous with the rest of the event data (i.e. 1978 and 1981) were excluded from the dynamic network. In order to determine whether the variables biasing network structure have changed over time, the analysis was conducted separately with the data from 1984-1999 and 2000-2017. For each time period a set of STERGM models with every possible combination of individual-level variables were fit to the dynamic network using conditional maximum likelihood estimation (CMLE). Although STERGM can be used to separately model both the formation and dissolution of links, this analysis was restricted to the former. Gender, popularity, and followers were included to investigate homophily, while mean distance was included to assess its effect on link formation. As STERGM cannot be run with missing covariates, NA values in popularity (6.39%), followers (6.39%), and mean distance (38.49%) were imputed using the random forest method. The models from each period were ranked according to AIC, and the best fitting models (ΔAIC < 2) with the most individual-level variables were run separately to assess the effects of each variable on network evolution.

Results

The three most heavily sampled drum breaks of all time were collectively sampled 6530 times (n1 = 2966, n2 = 2099, n3 = 1465). 4462 (68.33%) of these sampling events were associated with valid Discogs IDs, corresponding to 2432 unique artists (F: n = 143, 5.88%; M: n = 1342, 55.18%; Other or NA: n = 947, 38.94%), and included in the primary OADA and STERGM. The eight samples released after 1990 were collectively sampled 1752 times (n1 = 284, n2 = 260, n3 = 248, n4 = 198, n5 = 194, n6 = 193, n7 = 192, n8 = 182). 1305 (74.53%) of these sampling events were associated with valid Discogs IDs, corresponding to 1270 unique artists, and included in the additional OADA.

NBDA

The best fitting model from the primary OADA, which was multiplicative and included all four individual-level variables, can be seen in Table 1. In support of our first hypothesis, a likelihood ratio test found strong evidence for social transmission over asocial learning (ΔAICc = 141; p < 0.001). Based on the effect sizes, transmission appears to be more likely among females (p < 0.01) and less likely among artists who are more popular (p < 0.001) and have more followers (p < 0.001). Mean distance is not a significant predictor of transmission (p = 0.89). The diffusion network and diffusion curve for all three drum breaks included in the primary OADA are shown in Fig 1 and S1 Fig, respectively. All other models fit to the primary OADA can be found in S1 Appendix.

thumbnail
Table 1. The results of the multiplicative model for the OADA including all individual-level variables.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0211860.t001

thumbnail
Fig 1. The diffusion of all three drum breaks through the combined collaboration network.

At each time point individuals who have not yet used one of the drum breaks (informed) are shown in white, individuals who first used one of the drum breaks in a previous time step (previously informed) are shown in blue, and individuals who first used one of the drum breaks in the current time step (newly informed) are shown in red.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0211860.g001

The results of the additional OADA, conducted using the seven diffusions from after 1990, can be found in S1 Appendix. A likelihood ratio test found strong evidence for social transmission overall (ΔAICc = 88; p < 0.001). Contrary to our second hypothesis, linear regression found no significant relationships between either mean year of diffusion and social transmission estimate (R2 = 0.20, p = 0.31) or median year of diffusion and social transmission estimate (R2 = 0.17, p = 0.36) (see S2 Fig).

STERGM

For both time periods the second best fitting STERGM models (ΔAIC < 2) included all four individual-level variables, the results of which can be seen in Table 2. All other models can be found in S1 Appendix. Across both periods there appears to be homophily based on popularity (p < 0.001) and gender (M: p < 0.001; F: ps < 0.05). In support of our third hypothesis, mean distance negatively predicts link formation only before 2000 (p < 0.001). Additionally, there is a heterophilic effect of followers only after 2000 (p < 0.001). Based on the effect sizes, there has been a nearly three-fold decrease in the strength of homophily among females. Conversely, the strengh of homophily by popularity has actually increased since 2000. The results of the STERGM analysis assuming different transition years (e.g. 1994, 1996, 1998, 2002, 2004, 2006) can be found in S1 Appendix. Linear regression found significant positive relationships between both popularity and number of collaborations (R2 = 0.048, p < 0.001) and followers and number of collaborations (R2 = 0.090, p < 0.001) (see S3 Fig).

thumbnail
Table 2. The results of the STERGM analyses for before and after 2000.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0211860.t002

A goodness-of-fit analysis was conducted by generating simulated networks (n = 100) from the parameters of the best fitting model and comparing them to the observed network statistics [42]. For both time periods, the global statistics (i.e. gender, popularity, followers, mean distance) from the simulated networks were not significantly different from those observed, indicating that both models are good fits for the variables in question. Structural statistics (i.e. degree, edgewise shared partner, minimum geodesic distance) from the simulated networks were significantly different from those observed, indicating that both models are not good fits for the structural properties of the network. The results of this analysis can be found in S1 Appendix.

Discussion

Using high-resolution collaboration and longitudinal diffusion data, we have provided the first quantitative evidence that music samples are culturally transmitted via collaboration between artists. Additionally, in support of the widespread assertion that the internet has delocalized artist communities, we have found evidence that geographic proximity no longer biases the structure of musical collaboration networks after the year 2000. Given that the strength of transmission has not weakened over the same time period, this finding indicates that collaboration remains a key cultural transmission mode for music sampling traditions. This result supports the idea that the internet has enhanced rather than disrupted existing social interactions [9].

Gender appears to play a key role in both network structure and cultural transmission. Across the entire time period, collaborations were more likely to occur between individuals of the same gender. Additionally, the probability of cultural transmission appears to be much higher for female artists. This effect could be a result of the much higher levels of homophily among women before 2000. Previous work has suggested that high levels of gender homophily are associated with gender disparity [4345], which is consistent with the historic marginalization of women in music production communities [1, 10, 46]. Although the proportion of female artists in the entire dataset is extremely low (∼6%), the reduction in homophily among female artists after 2000 could be reflective of increasing inclusivity [47].

Artists with similar levels of popularity were also more likely to collaborate with each other. The increase in homophily by popularity after 2000 could be the result of an increase in skew, whereby fewer artists take up a greater proportion of the music charts [48]. In addition, the probability of cultural transmission appears to be higher among less popular artists, even though they are slightly less collaborative. This effect could be linked to cultural norms within “underground” music production communities. In these communities, collective cultural production is sometimes prioritized over individual recognition [49, 50]. This principle is best demonstrated by the historic popularity of the white-label release format, where singles are pressed to blank vinyl and distributed without artist information [49, 50]. In more extreme cases, individual artists who experience some level of mainstream success or press coverage risk losing credibility, and may even be perceived as undermining the integrity of their music community [50, 51]. Concerns about credibility could cause individuals to selectively copy less popular artists or utilize more rare samples (i.e. De La Soul’s refusal to sample James Brown and George Clinton because of their use by other popular groups [52]). Future research should investigate whether the “high prestige attached to obscurity” [50] in these communities may be driving a model-based bias for samples used by less popular artists or a frequency-based bias for samples that are more rare in the population [53]. A frequency-based novelty bias was recently identified in Western classical music using agent-based modeling [31], and similar methods could be utilized for sampling.

Similarly to popularity, the number of followers an individual has negatively predicts transmission probability. However, artists with similar numbers of followers were actually less likely to collaborate with each other after 2000. This result could be due to the fact that followers is a better indicator of current popularity, but has lower resolution further back in time. Newer artists with inflated follower counts who collaborate with older, historically-important artists with lower follower counts may still be expressing homophily based on overall popularity.

There are several limitations to this study that should be highlighted. Firstly, the time lag inherent in the user editing of WhoSampled means that older transmission records are more complete. Algorithms for sample-detection [54] may allow researchers to reconstruct full transmission records in the future, but these approaches are not yet publicly available. Secondly, MusicBrainz and Spotify had incomplete demographic data for some artists (i.e. gender and geographic location), which may have introduced bias into our model estimates. Lastly, Discogs primarily documents official releases, which means that more recent releases on streaming sites like Soundcloud are not well-represented. In combination with the exclusion of artists without Discogs IDs, this indicates that less prominent artists may be underrepresented in the dataset. Fortunately, social networks are fairly robust to missing data, especially when networks are large, centralized, and disproportionately include central nodes [5557]. Additionally, simulation studies evaluating the robustness of NBDA indicate that it performs well under fairly high levels of sampling error and bias [34, 5860], risks that are mitigated by the fact that the network was reconstructed from published collaborations rather than temporal co-occurrence data. Data on collaborative relationships between artists are less likely to suffer from significant observation error because (1) they do not have to be statistically transformed or filtered before network construction and (2) they result in cultural artifacts that are part of the public record. The crowd-sourced nature of these data is unlikely to impact the results, given that comparative studies of other crowd-sourced, quantitative datasets indicate that they have high accuracy [6163].

The results of this study provide valuable insight into how demographic variables, particularly gender and popularity, have biased both cultural transmission and the evolution of collaboration networks going into the digital age. In addition, we provide evidence that collaboration remains a key transmission mode of music sampling traditions despite the delocalization of communities by the internet. Future research should investigate whether decreased homophily among females is actually linked to greater inclusivity in the music industry (e.g. booking rates, financial compensation, media coverage), as well as whether the inverse effect of popularity on cultural transmission probability is a result of a model-based bias for obscurity or a frequency-based bias for novelty.

Supporting information

S1 Appendix. Statistical output, goodness-of-fit tests, and supplementary analyses.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0211860.s001

(PDF)

S1 Fig. The combined diffusion curve for all three drum breaks included in the primary OADA.

The proportion of informed individuals is on the y-axis, and the year is on the x-axis. Although recent research suggests that inferring acquisition modes from diffusion curves is unreliable, it appears that the curve may have the S-shape indicative of social transmission prior to the early-2000s.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0211860.s002

(TIFF)

S2 Fig. The relationship between diffusion years and transmission strengths for all seven diffusions included in the additional OADA.

The mean (left) and median (right) years of diffusion are on the x-axis, and the social transmission estimates from the additive model are on the y-axis. Linear regression found no significant relationships between either mean year of diffusion and social transmission estimate (R2 = 0.20, p = 0.31) or median year of diffusion and social transmission estimate (R2 = 0.17, p = 0.36).

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0211860.s003

(TIFF)

S3 Fig. The relationship between popularity and followers and the number of collaborations for each artist in the dataset.

Popularity and followers are on the x-axis, and number of collaborations is on the y-axis. Linear regression found significant positive relationships between both popularity and number of collaborations (R2 = 0.048, p < 0.001) and followers and number of collaborations (R2 = 0.090, p < 0.001).

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0211860.s004

(TIFF)

Acknowledgments

I would like to thank David Lahti and Carolyn Pytte, as well as all members of the Lahti lab, for their valuable conceptual and analytical feedback.

References

  1. 1. Whelan AM. The “Amen” Breakbeat as Fratriarchal Totem. In: Gender and Music. Heidelberg: Universitatsverlag Winter; 2009. p. 111–133.
  2. 2. Collins S. Amen to that. M/C Journal. 2007;10(2).
  3. 3. Frane AV. Swing Rhythm in Classic Drum Breaks From Hip-Hop’s Breakbeat Canon. Music Perception. 2017;34(3):291–302.
  4. 4. Väkevä L. Garage band or GarageBand®? Remixing musical futures. British Journal of Music Education. 2010;27(1):59–70.
  5. 5. Rodgers T. On the process and aesthetics of sampling in electronic music production. Organised Sound. 2003;8(3):313–320.
  6. 6. Bown O, Eldridge A, McCormack J. Understanding interaction in contemporary digital music: From instruments to behavioural objects. Organised Sound. 2009;14(2):188–196.
  7. 7. Lloyd R. Unauthorized Digital Sampling in the Changing Music Landscape. Journal of Intellectual Property Law. 2014;22(1):143–176.
  8. 8. Bodiford JR. Sharing Sounds: Musical Innovation, Collaboration, and Ideological Expression in the Chilean Netlabel Movement. University of Michigan; 2017.
  9. 9. DiMaggio P, Hargittai E, Neuman WR, Robinson JP. Social Implications of the Internet. Annual Review of Sociology. 2001;27:307–336.
  10. 10. Ebare S. The Computerization of Practice in Peripheral Music Communities. Simon Fraser University; 2003.
  11. 11. Alexandraki C, Valsamakis N. Enabling virtual music performance communities. In: Virtual Community Practices and Social Interactive Media: Technology Lifecycle and Workflow Analysis. Hershey: IGI Global; 2009. p. 378–399.
  12. 12. Salavuo M. Open and informal online communities as forums of collaborative musical activities and learning. British Journal of Music Education. 2006;23(3):253–271.
  13. 13. Lazar J, Preece J. Social Considerations in Online Communities: Usability, Sociability, and Success Factors; 2002.
  14. 14. Garg R, Smith MD, Telang R. Measuring Information Diffusion in an Online Community. Journal of Management Information Systems. 2011;28(2):11–38.
  15. 15. Florida R, Jackson S. Sonic city: The evolving economic geography of the music industry. Journal of Planning Education and Research. 2010;29(3):310–321.
  16. 16. Graham S. Global and Local Underground/Fringe Scenes. In: Sounds of the Underground. University of Michigan Press; 2016. p. 28–50.
  17. 17. Kruse H. Local Identity and Independent Music Scenes, Online and Off. Popular Music and Society. 2010;33(5):625–639.
  18. 18. Makelberge N. Rethinking collaboration in networked music. Organised Sound. 2012;17(1):28–35.
  19. 19. Ellis BK, Hwang H, Savage PE, Pan BY, Cohen AJ, Brown S. Identifying style-types in a sample of musical improvisations using dimensional reduction and cluster analysis. Psychology of Aesthetics, Creativity, and the Arts. 2018;12(1):110–122.
  20. 20. Savage PE, Brown S. Mapping Music: Cluster Analysis Of Song-Type Frequencies Within And Between Cultures. Ethnomusicology. 2014;58(1):133–155.
  21. 21. Mauch M, Maccallum RM, Levy M, Leroi AM, Mauch M. The evolution of popular music: USA 1960–2010. Royal Society Open Science. 2015;2. pmid:26064663
  22. 22. Percino G, Klimek P, Thurner S. Instrumentational complexity of music genres and why simplicity sells. PLoS ONE. 2014;9(12):1–16.
  23. 23. Savage PE, Brown S, Sakai E, Currie TE. Statistical universals reveal the structures and functions of human music. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences. 2015;112(29):8987–8992.
  24. 24. Rodriguez Zivic PH, Shifres F, Cecchi GA. Perceptual basis of evolving Western musical styles. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences. 2013;110(24):10034–10038.
  25. 25. Schlitter N, Falkowski T. Mining the Dynamics of Music Preferences from a Social Networking Site. In: 2009 International Conference on Advances in Social Network Analysis and Mining. Athens, Greece; 2009.
  26. 26. Monechi B, Gravino P, Servedio VDP, Tria F, Loreto V. Significance and popularity in music production. Royal Society Open Science. 2017;4(7). pmid:28791169
  27. 27. Verhoef T. The origins of duality of patterning in artificial whistled languages. Language and Cognition. 2012;4(4):357–380. pmid:23637710
  28. 28. Ravignani A, Delgado T, Kirby S. Musical evolution in the lab exhibits rhythmic universals. Nature Human Behaviour. 2017;1.
  29. 29. Lumaca M, Baggio G. Cultural transmission and evolution of melodic structures in multi-generational signaling games. Artificial Life. 2017;23:406–423. pmid:28786724
  30. 30. Rossman G, Chiu MM, Mol JM. Modeling diffusion of multiple innovations via multilevel diffusion curves: Payola in pop music radio. Sociological Methodology. 2008;38:201–230.
  31. 31. Nakamura E, Kaneko K. Statistical Evolutionary Laws in Music Styles. ArXiv e-prints. 2018;.
  32. 32. Acerbi A. A cultural evolution approach to digital media. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience. 2016;10(December):636. pmid:28018200
  33. 33. Laland KN, Kendal JR. What the models say about social learning. In: Fragaszy DM, Perry S, editors. The Biology of Traditions: Models and Evidence. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press; 2003. p. 33–55.
  34. 34. Franz M, Nunn CL. Network-based diffusion analysis: a new method for detecting social learning. Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences. 2009;276(1663):1829–1836. pmid:19324789
  35. 35. Aplin LM, Farine DR, Morand-Ferron J, Cockburn A, Thornton A, Sheldon BC. Experimentally induced innovations lead to persistent culture via conformity in wild birds. Nature. 2014;518(7540):538–541. pmid:25470065
  36. 36. Allen J, Weinrich M, Hoppitt W, Rendell L. Network-Based Diffusion Analysis Reveals Cultural Transmission of Lobtail Feeding in Humpback Whales. Science. 2013;340(6131):485–488. pmid:23620054
  37. 37. Hobaiter C, Poisot T, Zuberbühler K, Hoppitt W, Gruber T. Social Network Analysis Shows Direct Evidence for Social Transmission of Tool Use in Wild Chimpanzees. PLoS Biology. 2014;12(9):e1001960. pmid:25268798
  38. 38. Krivitsky PN, Handcock MS. A separable model for dynamic networks. Journal of the Royal Statistical Society. 2010;76(1):29–46.
  39. 39. Aplin LM, Farine DR, Morand-Ferron J, Sheldon BC. Social networks predict patch discovery in a wild population of songbirds. Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences. 2012;279(1745):4199–4205. pmid:22915668
  40. 40. Hoppitt W, Boogert NJ, Laland KN Detecting social transmission in networks. Journal of Theoretical Biology. 2010;263(4):544–555. pmid:20064530
  41. 41. Burnham KP, Anderson DR. Model selection and multimodel inference: a practical information-theoretic approach. 2nd ed. Springer; 2002.
  42. 42. Hunter DR, Goodreau SM, Handcock MS. Goodness of Fit of Social Network Models. Journal of the American Statistical Association. 2008;103(481):248–258.
  43. 43. Glass C, Cook A. Do Women Leaders Promote Positive Change? Analyzing the Effect of Gender on Business Practices and Diversity Initiatives. Human Resource Management. 2017; p. 1–15.
  44. 44. Crewe L, Wang A. Gender inequalities in the City of London advertising industry. Environment and Planning A: Economy and Space. 2018;50(3):671–688.
  45. 45. Jadidi M, Karimi F, Lietz H, Wagner C. Gender Disparities in Science? Dropout, Productivity, Collaborations and Success of Male and Female Computer Scientists. Advances in Complex Systems. 2018;21.
  46. 46. Baker S, Cohen BMZ. From snuggling and snogging to sampling and scratching: Girls’ nonparticipation in community-based music activities. Youth & Society. 2008;39(3):316–339.
  47. 47. Smith JA, McPherson M, Smith-Lovin L. Social Distance in the United States: Sex, Race, Religion, Age, and Education Homophily among Confidants, 1985 to 2004. American Sociological Review. 2014;79(3):432–456.
  48. 48. Ordanini A, Nunes JC. Fewer blockbusters and more superstars: How technological innovation has impacted convergence on the music charts. International Journal of Research in Marketing. 2016;33(2):297–313.
  49. 49. Thornton S. Club Cultures: Music, Media and Subcultural Capital. Cambridge: Polity Press; 1995.
  50. 50. Hesmondhalgh D. The British Dance Music Industry: A Case Study of Independent Cultural Production. The British Journal of Sociology. 1998;49(2):234–251.
  51. 51. Noys B. Into the ‘Jungle’. Popular Music. 1995;14(3):321–332.
  52. 52. Lena JC. Meaning and membership: samples in rap music, 1979-1995. Poetics. 2004;32:297–310.
  53. 53. Boyd R, Richerson PJ. Culture and the Evolutionary Process. Chicago: The University of Chicago Press; 1985.
  54. 54. Hockman JA, Davies MEP. Computational Strategies for Breakbeat Classification and Resequencing in Hardcore, Jungle and Drum & Bass. In: Proc. of the 18th Int. Conference on Digital Audio Effects (DAFx-15); 2015.
  55. 55. Smith J, Moody J. Structural effects of network sampling coverage I: Nodes missing at random. Social Networks. 2013;35(4):652–668.
  56. 56. Smith J, Moody J, Morgan J. Network sampling coverage II: The effect of non-random missing data on network measurement. Social Networks. 2017;48:78–99. pmid:27867254
  57. 57. Silk MJ, Jackson AL, Croft DP, Colhoun K, Bearhop S. The consequences of unidentifiable individuals for the analysis of an animal social network. Animal Behaviour. 2015;104.
  58. 58. Whalen A, Hoppitt W. Bayesian model selection with Network Based Diffusion Analysis. Frontiers in Psychology. 2016;7.
  59. 59. Hoppitt W. The conceptual foundations of Network-Based Diffusion Analysis: choosing networks and interpreting results. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B. 2017;372.
  60. 60. Wild S, Hoppitt W. Choosing a sensible cut-off point: assessing the impact of uncertainty in a social network on the performance of NBDA. Primates. 2018. pmid:30302657
  61. 61. Rajagopalan MS, Khanna V, Stott M, Leiter Y, Showalter TN, Dicker A, et al. Accuracy of cancer information on the Internet: A comparison of a Wiki with a professionally maintained database. Journal of Clinical Oncology. 2010;28.
  62. 62. Brown AR. Wikipedia as a Data Source for Political Scientists: Accuracy and Completeness of Coverage. PS: Political Science & Politics. 2011;44(2):339–343.
  63. 63. Kräenbring J, Penza TM, Gutmann J, Muehlich S, Zolk O, Wojnowski L, et al. Accuracy and Completeness of Drug Information in Wikipedia: A Comparison with Standard Textbooks of Pharmacology. PLOS ONE. 2014;9(9). pmid:25250889