Skip to main content
Advertisement
Browse Subject Areas
?

Click through the PLOS taxonomy to find articles in your field.

For more information about PLOS Subject Areas, click here.

  • Loading metrics

The contexts of heavy drinking: A systematic review of the combinations of context-related factors associated with heavy drinking occasions

  • Oliver Stanesby ,

    Roles Conceptualization, Data curation, Formal analysis, Investigation, Project administration, Visualization, Writing – original draft, Writing – review & editing

    o.stanesby@latrobe.edu.au

    Affiliation Centre for Alcohol Policy Research, La Trobe University, Melbourne, Australia

  • Florian Labhart ,

    Contributed equally to this work with: Florian Labhart, Paul Dietze, Cassandra J. C. Wright, Emmanuel Kuntsche

    Roles Conceptualization, Formal analysis, Project administration, Supervision, Writing – review & editing

    Affiliations Centre for Alcohol Policy Research, La Trobe University, Melbourne, Australia, Idiap Research Institute, Martigny, Switzerland, Addiction Switzerland, Research Institute, Lausanne, Switzerland

  • Paul Dietze ,

    Contributed equally to this work with: Florian Labhart, Paul Dietze, Cassandra J. C. Wright, Emmanuel Kuntsche

    Roles Supervision, Writing – review & editing

    Affiliations Burnet Institute, Melbourne, Australia, School of Public Health and Preventive Medicine, Monash University, Melbourne, Australia

  • Cassandra J. C. Wright ,

    Contributed equally to this work with: Florian Labhart, Paul Dietze, Cassandra J. C. Wright, Emmanuel Kuntsche

    Roles Conceptualization, Formal analysis, Writing – review & editing

    Affiliations Centre for Alcohol Policy Research, La Trobe University, Melbourne, Australia, Burnet Institute, Melbourne, Australia, School of Public Health and Preventive Medicine, Monash University, Melbourne, Australia

  • Emmanuel Kuntsche

    Contributed equally to this work with: Florian Labhart, Paul Dietze, Cassandra J. C. Wright, Emmanuel Kuntsche

    Roles Conceptualization, Funding acquisition, Project administration, Supervision, Writing – review & editing

    Affiliations Centre for Alcohol Policy Research, La Trobe University, Melbourne, Australia, Behavioural Science Institute, Radboud University, Nijmegen, The Netherlands

Abstract

Background

The amount of alcohol consumed during an occasion can be influenced by physical and social attributes of the setting, characteristics and state of individuals, and the interactions of these components. This systematic review identifies and describes the specific combinations and sequences of context-related factors that are associated with heavy drinking occasions.

Materials and methods

We conducted a systematic literature search of MEDLINE, Embase and the Cumulative Index to Nursing and Allied Health Literature (CINAHL) databases. Eligible articles were event-level and event-based studies that quantitatively analysed associations of sequences or combinations of context-related factors with event-level alcohol consumption. We extracted information on study design, sample, variables, effect estimates and analytical methods. We compiled a list of combinations and sequences associated with heavier drinking (i.e., ‘risky contexts’) and with lighter drinking (‘protective contexts’). The review protocol was registered with PROSPERO (registration number: CRD42018089500).

Results

We screened 1902 retrieved records and identified a final sample of 65 eligible studies. Daily mood, day of week, location and drinking group characteristics are important drivers of whether an individual engages in a heavy drinking occasion. The direction and magnitude of some associations differed by gender, age, personality and motives, such that in particular social or physical contexts, some people may feel compelled to drink more while others are compelled to drink less. Very few sequences of factors were reported as being associated with event-level alcohol consumption.

Conclusions

Contexts or factors are experienced in specific sequences that shape the broader drinking context and influence drinking behaviours and consequences but are under-studied. Event-level studies such as those using ecological momentary assessment can harness new technologies for data collection and analysis to improve understandings of why people engage in heavy drinking. Continued event-level research will facilitate public health interventions and policies that reduce heavy drinking and alcohol-related harms.

1. Introduction

Heavy drinking can result in harm at the individual, familial, community and societal levels [14]. Drinking tends to occur in specific contexts. The social and physical characteristics of contexts as well as the characteristics and state of the individual can influence whether they engage in heavy drinking and whether alcohol-related consequences are experienced [510]. Contextual factors may combine or co-occur during a drinking occasion in a specific manner that shapes the broader drinking context and influences an individuals’ drinking. A comprehensive review of the literature is needed to compile and summarise the specific contexts associated with heavy drinking patterns to reveal opportunities for effective environmental approaches to reduce alcohol-related harms due to heavy drinking.

1.1. Heavy drinking

Heavy drinking patterns (commonly termed ‘binge’, ‘risky single occasion’, ‘heavy episodic’ or ‘short-term risky’ drinking) involve consuming a relatively high amount of alcohol in a relatively short period of time [3, 11, 12]. Heavy drinking is most common on Friday and Saturday nights when young people go out and have few work or study responsibilities the following day [13, 14]. Short term consequences of heavy drinking include blackout, memory loss, nausea, vomiting, hangovers, alcohol poisoning, unintended and/or unprotected sexual activity, injury, traffic accidents, and death [2, 3, 11, 1517]. Those who engage in heavy drinking may also be more likely to harm others, for example via vandalization, inter-personal violence and aggression, and traffic accidents due to drink-driving [3, 4, 1822]. While heavy drinking is typically discussed in terms of immediate harms, consequences can also be long-term (e.g., permanent disability from an injury sustained during a traffic accident). Furthermore, there is evidence indicating heavy drinking in early life is linked to numerous long-term negative consequences [3, 11, 17, 23, 24].

Event-level alcohol consumption refers to an individuals’ drinking pattern during a given occasion. An occasion typically refers to a day or evening, but may be more specific (e.g., during a visit to a venue). Event-level alcohol consumption is distinct from measures of alcohol consumption across longer time-periods (e.g., average daily alcohol consumption in the last 12 months, usual alcohol consumption per drinking occasion in last week). Unless otherwise stated, hereafter ‘drinking’ refers to event-level alcohol consumption, ‘heavy’ or ‘heavier’ drinking refers to higher event-level alcohol consumption, and ‘light’ or lighter’ drinking refers to lower event-level alcohol consumption.

1.2. Drinking occurs in complex contexts that influence drinking behaviour

According to a social ecological perspective of human behaviour [5, 6, 810], the immediate drinking context can be characterised by the physical and social attributes of the setting, the characteristics and state of individuals, and the interactions of these components. This model implies that contextual factors influence whether an individual engages in heavy drinking and whether alcohol-related consequences are experienced. Further, associations between individual-level factors (e.g., gender, age, personality and motives) and heavy drinking may be altered by contextual factors (e.g., time, place, occasion and presence of others).

1.3. The broader drinking context: a sequence of immediate drinking contexts

Previous studies have focused on the independent effects of factors on drinking occasions and consequences (e.g., [2528]. However, in the real world, factors may combine or co-occur during a drinking occasion in a specific sequential manner that shapes the broader drinking context and influences an individuals’ drinking. Fig 1 provides a visual depiction of the broader drinking context. As shown, the broader drinking context comprises a sequence of immediate drinking contexts that are described by combinations and sequences of factors related to the characteristics and state of individuals, the physical environment and the social environment. Pre-drinking or pre-loading provides an example of a risky sequential combination of context-related factors—the act of drinking alcohol, usually at a domestic residence, prior to attending a social event, typically at a bar or nightclub [29, 30]. Occasions that include pre-drinking are associated with heavier drinking than those that do not [29, 3134]. Moving between several outlets (e.g., pub crawling) provides another example of a risky combination of sequential factors.

thumbnail
Fig 1. The broader drinking context: Comprising a sequence of immediate drinking contexts which are described by combinations of factors related to the characteristics and state of individuals, the physical environment and the social environment 1.

1 As per a social ecological perspective of human behaviour [5, 6, 810].

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0218465.g001

Individual-level factors may also moderate associations between sequences of factors and drinking; for example, the positive association between pre-drinking and likelihood of heavy drinking is stronger among men with lower conformity drinking motives than men with higher conformity motives [35].

The complexity of the broader drinking context (Fig 1) means that it is imperative for research to consider the interplay (i.e., interaction or combined effect) of multiple factors relating to the context and the individual (rather than simply estimating the independent effect of one factor), as well as the specific sequence of these factors. This is necessary to comprehensively explain associations between drinking contexts and heavy drinking and alcohol-related harms.

1.4. Studying drinking contexts and event-level associations

Two main types of studies have been used to investigate simultaneous and/or prospective relationships of context-related factors and individual-level factors with drinking. The first is event-level studies, which collect data during the event(s) using methods such as ecological momentary assessment (EMA). The second is event-based studies, which collect data retrospectively about an event (e.g., retrospective survey) or events (e.g., timeline follow back; TLFB). In theory, both designs allow the exploration of event-level associations between specific contexts and heavy drinking. However, their ability to comprehensively describe contexts and explore event-level associations between contexts and drinking varies.

Some event-level and event-based studies have identified specific combinations of factors related to drinking contexts and/or individuals that are linked to heavier drinking occasions among adolescents and young adults. For example, Thrul and Kuntsche found that the positive association between drinking with friends and heavier drinking was stronger among males than females [36]. Lau-Barraco and colleagues report that drinking occasions were particularly heavy on weekend days for those with higher social alcohol expectancies [37]. Another study reported that occasions spent in public locations with few intoxicated people are associated with lighter drinking [38].

1.5. The need for a review of the literature on the contexts of heavy drinking

To our knowledge, no review has comprehensively summarised the evidence on the specific contexts that are associated with heavy drinking. Further, we found no studies that comprehensively reviewed and discussed the design and methodology of these studies. A comprehensive review of the literature is needed to compile and summarise the specific contexts associated with heavy drinking patterns.

To address these gaps, we conducted a systematic review which:

  1. Summarises the immediate contexts (described by specific combinations of context-related and individual-level factors) and broader contexts (described by specific sequences of immediate contexts) that are associated with heavier drinking occasions. It provides a more comprehensive understanding of how contexts influence drinking, highlights gaps in understanding of contexts influencing heavy drinking and suggests opportunities for effective environmental approaches to reduce alcohol-related harms due to heavy drinking; and
  2. Summarises, critiques and proposes ways to improve the design and analytical methodology of the scientific literature that investigates event-level associations between contexts and drinking.

2. Materials and methods

2.1. Study identification, eligibility and screening

A systematic review of English articles using MEDLINE, Embase and the Cumulative Index to Nursing and Allied Health Literature (CINAHL) databases was conducted to identify eligible articles. The review protocol was registered with PROSPERO (registration number: CRD42018089500). The review was conducted in line with the PRISMA Statement [39] (see S1 Table for completed PRISMA checklist). Given the complexity of drinking contexts, this review targeted studies that provided descriptions of drinking contexts via sequences or combinations of multiple factors related to the context. Articles eligible for inclusion in data extraction and synthesis were those which: (a) included a quantitative analysis of event-level data; (b) estimated associations of sequences or combinations of two or more factors (at least one context-level variable) with event-level alcohol consumption of the individual; and (c) reported a combination or sequence that was directly associated with significantly increased (‘risky’) or decreased (‘protective’) event-level alcohol consumption, and (d) sampled from broadly Western countries (this criterion was added after the full-text screening phase and prior to data extraction because the majority of eligible studies sampled from Western countries). To maximise the comparability of the findings with general populations, studies with samples comprised only of those with alcohol use disorder (for example) were ineligible. Associations eligible for extraction (Table 1) were those which:

  1. statistically tested for differences in effect between groups of respondents described by combinations of factors (e.g., via t-test, odds ratio, beta coefficient, interaction), or it was possible to conservatively test via 95% confidence intervals, or reported the level of endorsement of responses in latent class analysis; and
  2. observed a statistically significant difference (increase or decrease) in the outcome at the 95% confidence level (e.g., p < 0.05 or, when p was not reported, 95% confidence intervals that do not include null or do not overlap), or heavy or lighter drinking endorsed by the majority of a class (>0.50 probability) in latent class analysis.
thumbnail
Table 1. Criteria for judging eligibility of retrieved articles for inclusion in data extraction and synthesis.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0218465.t001

Where studies presented both bivariate and multivariate estimates of a given association, only the effect estimates that are adjusted for potential confounding variables were extracted. For practical and conceptual reasons, qualitative studies and associations of factors with alcohol-related harms (but not event-level alcohol consumption) were not eligible for inclusion in this review. Further details about the eligibility criteria are described in Table 1.

The search strategy was designed in direct consultation with La Trobe University library staff with expertise in conducting systematic review literature searches. To ensure the search was highly sensitive to retrieving eligible records, we used a detailed list of search terms that describe alcohol drinking, event-level or event-based study design, and combinations, interactions or sequences. The search was conducted on the 29th of January 2018. A copy of the full search strategy is provided in S2 Table. Additional relevant articles were identified by contacting experts in this topic of research who suggested articles which were then screened for eligibility.

The process for screening records for eligibility is described in Fig 2. Titles and abstracts, then full texts of all retrieved records were screened by an independent researcher (OS) using the criteria described in Table 1. Records that OS was initially unsure whether to include or exclude were screened for eligibility by a second researcher (FL, CW or AP), and differences in opinions of eligibility were resolved via the majority opinion of a group of five researchers (OS, FL, CW, EK and AP).

thumbnail
Fig 2. Flow diagram of identification of eligible articles for review.

1 Of these, 15 were duplicates, 12 were excluded based on full text and 11 were included in quantitative synthesis; Records eligible for full text screening with no full text published in English were translated to English then screened; Date searched: 29 January 2018; Fig 2 adapted from: [39].

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0218465.g002

2.2. Data extraction and synthesis

The following information was extracted by OS from the full text of each article included for review:

  1. Type of study design: e.g., EMA, daily diary study, TLFB survey, intercept survey, retrospective survey.
  2. Sample description: e.g., N; adult, student, patron of night-time precinct, drinker; country; age range.
  3. Outcome variable description: e.g., number of drinks consumed, breath alcohol concentration (BrAC), heavy drinking episode; continuous or categorical.
  4. Predictor variable(s) description: e.g., pre-drank, number of friends present, gender; continuous or categorical; event-level (e.g., today) or individual-level (generally); sequence or combination.
  5. Type and magnitude of effect estimate(s) and comparison statistic(s): e.g., difference in means or proportions between groups described by combinations of factors, odds ratio, beta-coefficient, interaction coefficient; p-value, 95% confidence interval, probability.
  6. Analytical approach used: e.g., multi-level modelling, person-mean centering of event-level predictors, latent class analysis, factor analysis or principal components analysis.
  7. Direction of association(s) (for each valid association): e.g., heavier drinking (i.e., risky context) or lighter drinking (i.e., protective context).

The Quality Assessment Tool for Observational Cohort and Cross-Sectional Studies [40] was used by one researcher (OS) to identify potential sources of bias in the design or conduct of the study.

Combinations and sequences that were consistently associated with heavier drinking were compiled into a list summarising ‘risky contexts’ for men, for women and for either gender. Similarly, combinations and sequences associated with lighter drinking were compiled into a list of ‘protective contexts’ for men, women and either. Contexts were divided into categories that denoted whether the context was described by factors relating to the characteristics or state of the individual, physical environments and/or social environments. Specific combinations or sequences that were not unidirectionally associated with the outcome across studies (e.g., a combination was associated with heavier drinking in one study, but lighter drinking in another) were dropped (see S3 Table for dropped associations). A meta-analysis was not conducted because few studies investigated comparable combinations and sequences, thus yielding effect estimates that were not collapsible across a sufficient number of studies.

3. Results

3.1. Study selection

The flow of records throughout the screening process is presented in Fig 2. The literature database searches retrieved a total of 1,953 records. A further 38 records were retrieved via consultation with experts (‘other sources’). After the removal of 89 duplicate records, the titles and abstracts of 1,902 records were screened according to the eligibility criteria (Table 1). A further 1,661 records were excluded during the screening of titles and abstracts. Of the remaining 241 records which underwent full text screening, 163 were excluded during the full text screening and a further 13 were excluded during critical appraisal or data extraction. The reasons for exclusion during title and abstract screening, full text screening, and critical appraisal and data extraction are listed in Fig 2. The remaining 65 articles were included in the review.

3.2. Study descriptions and methodologies

3.2.1. Study sample.

Characteristics of the studies included in the review are described in Table 2. The sample size ranged from 47 to 60,215 individuals. However, it is important to note that EMA, daily diary and TLFB [41] designs produce observations for multiple occasions per individual, and EMA designs produce observations for multiple timepoints per occasion per individual. Thus, despite having a low sample size, the data from the EMA, daily diary and TLFB studies included in this review typically encapsulated hundreds of occasions or more. The majority (52/65; 80%) of the studies used data from samples comprised mostly or entirely of adolescents or young adults: students (36/65; 55%); nightlife precinct patrons (12/65; 18%); and people aged < 30 years who were not specifically students or nightlife precinct patrons (5/65; 8%). Twenty-nine studies (45%) restricted their sample to drinkers. Almost all of the studies drew their sample from populations in North America (42/65; 65%) and Europe (17/65; 26%). Participants were primarily of white/Caucasian ethnicity.

thumbnail
Table 2. Study characteristics and risk of bias within studies.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0218465.t002

3.2.2. Study design and analysis.

Five different types of study designs were included in the review. Eleven (17%) used a retrospective survey of a single previous drinking occasion, eight (12%) used a TLFB survey design, 14 (22%) used street interviews, 15 (23%) used daily diaries and 17 (26%) used an EMA design. Most studies (47/65; 72%) tested associations via multi-level modelling, accounting for the nested structure of the data, i.e., occasions (level 1) nested within individuals (level 2). Twenty-one studies (32%) used person-mean centered occasion-level predictor variables to aid interpretation of observed effects. Five studies (8%) constructed combinations of factors via latent class analysis or factor analysis.

The study quality (risk of within-studies bias) for most of the studies were rated ‘good’ (44/65; 68%), 20 (31%) were rated ‘fair’, and one (2%) was rated ‘poor’.

3.2.3. Outcomes.

Most studies (40/65; 62%) derived an outcome from the number of drinks consumed during the drinking occasion or that day or evening, or from estimated blood alcohol concentration (19/65; 29%). Two studies (3%) combined total drinks consumed during the occasion with ratings of intoxication to derive an outcome measure for intoxication level. Thirteen studies (20%) derived outcome variables from the number of drinks consumed at specific locations/settings (e.g., on premise, off premise, at home, away from home, alone, with others), until specific time-points (e.g., until nightclub entry) or during specific time-frames (e.g., 8pm-9pm, 9pm-10pm, etc.), or speed of consumption (e.g., drinking rate or drinking rate acceleration).

3.3. Synthesis of results: Contexts that encourage and discourage heavy drinking

The contexts that were found to be associated with heavier drinking are summarised in Table 3 (denoted via ↑). Contexts associated with lighter drinking are denoted via ↓.

thumbnail
Table 3. Contexts–described by combinations and sequences of factors related to the characteristics or state of the individual, the physical environment and the social environment 1 –associated with heavier drinking (↑) and lighter drinking (↓) 2.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0218465.t003

3.3.1. Coverage of contextual elements.

A total of 156 unique contexts were identified as being associated with heavier or lighter drinking. Of these, 110 contexts (71%) were associated with heavier drinking (labelled as ‘risky contexts’), and 46 contexts (29%) were associated with lighter drinking (labelled as ‘protective contexts’).

Twenty-eight studies (43%) investigated the association between a sequence of event-level factors and event-level drinking, and 52 studies (80%) investigated the association between a combination of two or more context-related factors and event-level drinking. The number of studies that constructed sequences or combinations from factors from the following domains were as follows:

  • individual characteristics/state only, 11/65 (17);
  • physical environment only, 2/65 (3%);
  • social environment only, 25/60 (38%);
  • individual characteristics/state and physical environment, 7/65 (11%);
  • individual characteristics/state and social environment, 21/65 (32%);
  • physical environment and social environment, 8/65 (12%);
  • individual characteristics/state and physical environment and social environment, 8/65 (12%).

Most contexts were described from combinations or sequences of factors related to the individual (128/156; 82%) or social environment (113/156; 72%), exclusively or in combination with individual, social environment or physical environment factors. Fewer contexts were described from factors related to the physical environment, exclusively or in combination with individual or social environment factors (68/156; 44%). Twenty-four risky contexts and thirteen protective contexts (total 37/156; 24%) were described from a combination of factors related to all three elements of a context: the individual, the social environment and the physical environment.

3.3.2. Individual’s state (e.g., daily mood).

Negative or positive states of mood were found to be associated with heavier or lighter drinking depending on the individual’s traits and environmental characteristics that these emotions are combined with [57, 61, 63, 72, 82, 86, 93]. Heavy drinking is particularly likely to occur on days when a lot of negative emotion or negative interpersonal events are experienced for individuals who are socially anxious [80], have low self-esteem, high shame or high neuroticism [54, 73, 77], have high social support [43], are less educated [61], have high drinking-to-cope motives [78, 82] or have low drinking to conform motives [78]. In contrast, studies suggested that people with low shame [73], non-students [86], students not affiliated with a sorority [72], men with low attention spans [57] and women with high attention spans [57] are less likely to engage in heavy drinking on days they have negative mood. Also, individuals with low social anxiety or low drinking to cope motives tend to drink less on days they experience negative interpersonal interactions [78, 80]. Individuals with relatively high self-esteem [54], high perceived ability to cope with distress [95] or who are older [66] are particularly likely to engage in heavy drinking on days when they are in a positive mood or experience positive interpersonal events.

3.3.3. Social characteristics of contexts (e.g., the drinking group).

The immediate social context was found to be most strongly associated with drinking behaviour when combined with certain individual characteristics. Heavy drinking occasions are more likely on occasions when many friends are present [36, 42, 47, 48, 52, 60, 62, 70, 78, 94, 96]–particularly for those with high attention to social aspects of drinking (i.e., cognitive bias towards social vs. non-social drinking situations) [60], when drinking at home [47, 78], for men with high coping drinking motives or low conformity motives when many female friends are present [94], for men with low coping motives when many male friends are present [94], for women with high enhancement motives when many male friends are present [94], and for women who have a cognitive bias for social alcohol-related stimuli when many female friends are present [60]. Drinking with a group of friends was associated with very heavy drinking when the group’s expectations of drinking and illicit drug use are high [48]. Logically, when the group expectations of drinking and illicit drug use are lower, this was associated with lighter drinking [48]. Having less friends present was identified as a particularly strong protective context for men in general [7], and for women who have a cognitive bias for non-social alcohol-related stimuli [60].

Certain contexts appear particularly conducive to heavy drinking on weekends. Generally, spending time with friends, particularly in large groups of friends, is associated with heavier drinking [42, 79]. On average, women, individuals with social expectancies related to alcohol, and older college students (fourth year vs. first year college students) who are generally in a positive mood are likely to drink much heavier on weekends than weekdays. On a day-to-day basis, older college students are particularly likely to drink heavily on weekend days if they are in a positive mood that day [63].

Some contexts experienced at a given time of the week are associated with lighter than usual drinking. Students who initiated drinking at an early age are less likely to engage in heavy drinking on weekend days if they are in a negative mood [63]. Also, engaging in spiritual activities on weekdays and engaging in athletics activities such as sports on weekends were contexts associated with lighter drinking than usual on weekdays and weekends, respectively [59].

3.3.4. Physical characteristics of contexts (e.g., location).

In particular circumstances, both public locations (e.g., bars) and private places (e.g., homes) may be conducive to heavy drinking. Occasions spent with groups of friends or in environments where many people are intoxicated were associated with heavy drinking regardless of whether they were in a public or private location [38]. Generally, social events such as parties are relatively likely to involve heavy drinking [49, 53, 84, 85, 97]. The increased likelihood of engaging in heavy drinking when attending a party is particularly large when the individual is hosting an off-campus party [49], for men when attending a University-related event or party [85] and for women when attending a themed party [53]. In contrast, when men attend a restaurant they are relatively unlikely to drink heavily [7]. Heavy drinking was more likely when spending the evening at home in a negative mood or on days involving negative interpersonal exchanges, especially among highly neurotic or extraverted people [77]. Heavy drinking was more likely for those with high drinking to cope and drinking to enhance motives when spending the evening at home in a positive mood, or on days when spending a lot of time with friends [78].

3.3.5. Sequences of context-related factors.

There were three main sequences that are each found to be associated with heavy drinking: pre-drinking, yesterday’s alcohol consumption and multi-day accumulation of stress. Multiple studies found that heavy drinking was more likely on occasions involving pre-drinking than non-pre-drinking occasions [31, 3335, 4446, 50, 56, 58, 64, 67, 69, 7476, 83, 8791, 98]. The positive association between pre-drinking and heavy drinking was particularly strong for women when combined with drinking spirits [69], for men when with a mixed gender or all same-sex group (vs. all opposite-sex) [64, 69], for men when illicit drugs are consumed [87], for men when drinking wine or champagne [69], and for men with low drinking to conform motives [35]. Occasions when an individual pre-drinks were particularly conducive to heavy drinking when the on-premise venue attended is a bar, especially for younger adults [31]. This effect was stronger among women than men and among generally lighter than heavier drinkers. Heavy drinking was also more likely after sequences of stressful days, particularly among women in general or among men or women with a lower level of education [61]. Overall, aside from pre-drinking, very few sequences of factors were reported as being associated with heavier or lighter drinking.

4. Discussion

4.1. Associations between context and heavy drinking, policy implications, and opportunities for research

Elements of contexts that were commonly associated with heavier or lighter drinking occasions included a person’s mood throughout the day, the size, gender and expectations of the social group, the location where drinking takes place, and whether certain events or parties are attended. However, the strength and direction of associations between context-level factors and drinking differ according to the characteristics of the individual (e.g., their gender, age, personality and motives). The variety of interactions among the elements of a context demonstrate the complexity of relationships between contexts and drinking behaviours. In fact, in a given social or physical context, some people may feel compelled to continue or accelerate their drinking, whereas others may be influenced to slow down, drink less, or choose not to drink at all. Therefore, it is important to consider the physical, social and individual elements of a context when endeavouring to understand contextual influences on people’s drinking. This is also an important consideration for interventions designed at the individual-level. Identification of contexts that are risky or protective for people of a particular gender and age and with particular personality traits and drinking motives may help to design and implement effective policies for reducing heavy drinking occasions and related harms among specific sub-populations.

Identifying contexts that are associated with heavy drinking and contexts associated with lighter drinking is useful because it enables targeted interventions and policies, thus potentially reducing heavy drinking and alcohol-related harms. Concurrent discouragement of risky contexts (via policies, targeted interventions, health promotion and education) and encouragement of protective contexts (via the same avenues) may further reduce heavy drinking and alcohol-related harms.

The search strategy retrieved 31 studies that are well-suited to measuring sequences of factors across an occasion (17 EMA studies and 14 intercept surveys). However, very few studies investigated links between specific sequences during an occasion and event-level drinking, aside from pre-drinking. It is also possible that some eligible studies that investigate sequences of factors during an occasion and event-level drinking were not captured by the literature search. One of the advantages of EMA studies is they can record the time of specific occurrences across the course of a drinking occasion [99]. Therefore, new EMA studies or analysis of existing EMA data are needed to identify specific sequences that influence young adults to accelerate, maintain, decelerate, or cease drinking on a given occasion.

Relatively few contexts were described in relation to the physical environment. Research investigating whether and how physical contexts are associated with an individual’s drinking behaviour is needed because, in a practical sense, modification of the physical environment may be relatively feasible (e.g. luminosity, noise level and density restrictions, location-specific text message interventions) [100]. Technologies such as smartphone-based environmental measurement tools may be useful for measuring physical environments and investigating associations with heavy drinking behaviours [101103].

A minority of the studies included in this review described contexts via a combination of factors related to the individual, the social environment and the physical environment. Future event-level studies that consider all the main elements that comprise a context have the potential to improve understandings of how and why specific contexts can influence drinking behaviour.

4.2. Study design and analytical methodology: implications and opportunities for studying drinking contexts

Numerous types of event-level or event-based designs were used by the studies included in this review. However, their ability to comprehensively describe contexts and explore their associations with event-level drinking is varied.

To accurately distinguish between the contextual influences at the event-level and the individual-level, studies which capture information about multiple occasions, such as EMA and daily diary studies are advantageous because they can properly account for both inter- and intra-individual variations using multi-level modelling [104, 105]. Studies that account for inter- and intra-individual variations using multi-level modelling can explore the true effects of contexts as drivers of within-person variation in event-level drinking [104, 105]. Multi-level modelling also enables event-level predictor variables to be centered according to each individual’s mean measurement across all occasions (called person-mean centering). Person-mean centering uses the person’s usual behaviour as its own baseline, and standardizes the momentary behaviour by considering only the deviation from the usual behaviour [105].

Studies with longer recall periods, such as retrospective surveys about single occasions and TLFB surveys, are more susceptible to recall bias [106].

Event-level studies may harness new technologies such as smartphone environmental measurement tools and continuous objective monitoring of alcohol use to improve measurement of contextual factors and behaviours. Continuous objective monitoring of blood alcohol concentration in real time via objective measures such as transdermal sensors may reduce the risk of self-reporting biasing measurements of alcohol consumption and intoxication levels [107]. Smartphones may provide useful tools for objectively measuring other contextual factors and behaviours [100, 101, 103, 108]. Although, data gathered via built-in sensors, camera, microphone and other features are not without limitation and subjective self-report questionnaires may temporarily remain the most practical method for measuring many contextual factors and behaviours.

This review found that most studies that explore event-level associations between contexts and drinking were conducted among samples of adolescents and young adults, and often students. Therefore, many of the risky and protective contexts identified may generalise to younger populations, but not to older populations. The scope of this review was widened from young adults to all ages during the project conception and design phase, to attain a more-complete review of the relevant literature and to compare the representation of young samples and older samples in the literature on drinking contexts. The high representation of young samples in the literature appears warranted, given that heavy drinking occasions are most frequent and intense during late adolescence and early adulthood [3, 109, 110]. Although, a proportion of older adults are heavy drinkers and, recently in many countries, alcohol consumption levels have decreased in younger age groups but have been maintained or increased in some older age groups [1, 111114]. Thus, further exploration of event-level associations between contexts and drinking among older populations may be warranted to identify and discourage contexts associated with heavy drinking in middle-late adulthood. Further, as all studies included in the review sampled from Western countries, the results may generalisable to younger populations in Western countries only.

Similarly, the results of this review may only generalise to broad Western populations, not to socio-demographically-defined sub-populations, which are known to vary in alcohol use and problems. Therefore, research investigating event-level associations between contexts and heavy drinking in specific sub-populations of both Western and non-Western countries is necessary.

The intuitive way to construct variables and interpret model estimates may partly explain why fewer protective contexts were identified than risky contexts. The reference category of a categorical variable or the lower value of a continuous variable typically corresponds with a lower level of exposure–e.g., a categorical variable for pre-drinking with reference category ‘no’ and comparison category ‘yes’. Also, the format of effect estimates of regression models (e.g., odds ratios, beta coefficients) make it intuitive to interpret associations as the comparison category (or higher value) versus the reference category (or lower value)–e.g., ‘pre-drinking is associated with heavy drinking’. The same effect estimate could also be interpreted as the reference category versus the comparison category–e.g., ‘not pre-drinking is associated with lighter drinking’. Both interpretations are correct and provide a slightly different interpretation, although the former (e.g., comparison category vs. reference category) is more intuitive and commonly reported. While in some cases it is possible to derive protective contexts by taking the reverse of the reported association (e.g., ‘did not pre-drink’), we chose not to do so because this is generally not how estimates are interpreted and explained in literature. Thus, the tendency for lower values to be a variable’s reference category/value may partly explain why fewer protective contexts were identified than risky contexts.

This review identified many well-designed studies that investigated event-level associations between contexts and drinking using appropriate analytical methodology. However, some studies were highly susceptible to recall bias, and the absence of multi-level modelling in 18 studies meant some studies may not have properly accounted for between-person differences that may bias event-level effects [104, 105].

4.3. Strengths and limitations

Traditionally, most event-level and event-based studies of drinking contexts have focussed on describing independent associations between singular isolated context-related factors and drinking outcomes. This review excludes a section of the literature that does not conceptualise and measure contexts as combinations or sequences of factors. As drinking contexts are described by the complex interaction of factors related to the physical and social environment and the individuals within them [5, 6, 810], these associations were not eligible for inclusion in this review. Therefore, there are likely numerous studies not included in this review that take a simpler approach and identify context-related factors which are independently associated with heavy drinking. However, the focus of this review on more complex conceptualisation and measurement of drinking contexts provides a novel and useful review of the cutting-edge of event-level alcohol research. Given the complex conceptualisation of drinking context employed by this review, developing a search strategy that effectively captures the relevant literature was difficult and a potential limitation. The terms used to identify event-level or event-based study design did not list specific study designs (e.g., timeline follow back, ecological momentary assessment) (S2 Table). An objective of this review was to summarise and critique the study design of the event-level drinking literature, and inclusion of such terms would bias the search to capturing study designs that are well known to the researchers a-priori. Experimental studies were not included in this review because they do not represent real-world settings but provide another method for studying drinking contexts. Screening and data extraction were conducted by an independent researcher. To minimise this limitation, records that the researcher was initially unsure whether to include or exclude were screened for eligibility by a second researcher, and differences in opinions of eligibility were resolved via the majority opinion of a group of five researchers. While event-level alcohol consumption and intoxication (the review’s outcome measure) is associated with alcohol-related harms, this review does not explore direct associations with between contexts and alcohol-related harms. While qualitative literature was not included in this review, such studies provide clues as to the types of contexts that may be associated with heavy drinking and related harms. Therefore, there may be some context-related factors associated with occasions involving heavy drinking or harms not captured in this review that warrant further exploration.

4.4. Conclusions

This review found that the contexts an individual encounters on a given occasion are associated with how heavily they drink alcohol during that occasion. The direction and magnitude of these associations differ according to the gender, age, personality, motives and mental state of the individual, such that in a given social or physical context some people may feel compelled to continue or accelerate their drinking whereas others may be influenced to slow down, drink less, or choose not to drink at all. Contexts or factors are experienced in specific sequences that shape the broader drinking context and influence drinking behaviours and alcohol-related consequences across drinking occasions. However, risky contextual sequences are under-studied. Therefore, event-level alcohol research should prioritise improving understandings of the types and mechanisms of contextual sequences that are associated with heavy drinking and alcohol-related harms. New technologies such as smartphone environmental measurement tools and continuous objective monitoring of alcohol use and multi-level analytical methods are recommended to improve understandings of why people engage in heavy drinking. Continued research investigating event-level associations between contexts and heavy drinking will facilitate public health interventions and policies that reduce heavy drinking and alcohol-related harms.

Supporting information

S1 Table. PRISMA checklist.

a Of submitted version of manuscript document; n.p.: Not possible (see page 10: ‘a meta-analysis was not conducted because few studies investigated comparable combinations and sequences, thus yielding effect estimates that were not collapsible across a sufficient number of studies’); S1 Table adapted from: [1].

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0218465.s001

(DOCX)

S2 Table. Search strategy used to retrieve articles from scientific literature databases to be screened for eligibility.

Databases searched: MEDLINE, Embase and the Cumulative Index to Nursing and Allied Health Literature (CINAHL); Searched titles and abstracts of records; The above search terms were applied in MEDLINE, and replicated in Embase and CINAHL (via some minor adjustments); Search limits: Exclude MEDLINE records (applied to Embase and CINAHL searches only); Executed search strategy and retrieved articles on 29 January 2018.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0218465.s002

(DOCX)

S3 Table. Contexts–described by combinations and sequences of factors related to the characteristics or state of the individual, the physical environment and the social environment 1 –associated with heavier drinking (↑) or lighter drinking (↓) 2 that were dropped due to inconsistent observed direction of association across studies.

Heavier drinking: Higher event-level alcohol consumption; Lighter drinking: Lower event-level alcohol consumption; 1 Must include at least one context-level variable and must not include event-level alcohol consumption (the outcome); 2 Compared to the reference categories of categorical variables and/or the lower values of continuous variables (unless inverted or transformed), as derived by the authors; ‘Today/yesterday’: event-level variable; ‘Generally’: Individual-level variable; Single factor + arrow in ‘all’ column: sequence; All: among sample of men and women combined; Single factor + arrow in gender column: effect of that single factor greater for that gender than the other gender (i.e., gender interaction) (does not refer to effect of one factor among sample of women or men); Combinations/sequences allocated to the most relevant subsection according to the types of factors involved (subsections in italics).

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0218465.s003

(DOCX)

S1 File. PROSPERO International prospective register of systematic reviews review protocol.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0218465.s004

(PDF)

Acknowledgments

This work was supported by the Swiss National Science Foundation (SNSF; http://www.snf.ch/en) project “Dusk2Dawn: Characterizing Youth Nightlife Spaces, Activities, and Drinks” (CRSII5_173696) awarded to Prof Daniel Gatica-Perez (DG-P) and Prof Emmanuel Kuntsche (EK). Paul Dietze (PD) is the recipient of a National Health and Medical Research Council (NHMRC; https://www.nhmrc.gov.au/) Senior Research Fellowship (1136908). Cassandra JC Wright (CJCW) is the recipient of a National Health and Medical Research Council (NHMRC; https://www.nhmrc.gov.au/) Early Career Research Fellowship (1161246). We thank Steven Chang of the La Trobe University Library for his assistance designing the systematic literature search strategy. Oliver Stanesby would personally like to thank the Walter Street Group for their active, and festive, participation in activities, many of which aided the discussion of the enclosed findings.

References

  1. 1. World Health Organization. Global status report on alcohol and health: 2014. Geneva, Switzerland: World Health Organization; 2014.
  2. 2. Gmel G, Rehm J, Kuntsche E. Binge drinking in Europe: Definitions, epidemiology, and consequences. Sucht. 2003;49(2):105–16.
  3. 3. Kuntsche E, Kuntsche S, Thrul J, Gmel G. Binge drinking: Health impact, prevalence, correlates and interventions. Psychology and Health. 2017;32(8):976–1017. pmid:28513195
  4. 4. Laslett AM, Room R, Ferris J, Wilkinson C, Livingston M, Mugavin J. Surveying the range and magnitude of alcohol's harm to others in Australia. Addiction. 2011;106(9):1603–11. pmid:21438943
  5. 5. Freisthler B, Lipperman-Kreda S, Bersamin M, Gruenewald P. Tracking the when, where, and with whom of alcohol use: Integrating ecological momentary assessment and geospatial data to examine risk for alcohol-related problems. Alcohol Research: Current Reviews. 2014;36(1):29–38.
  6. 6. McCarty D. Environmental factors in substance abuse. In: Galizio M, Maisto S, editors. Determinants of substance abuse: Biological, psychological, and environmental factors. Boston, MA: Springer US; 1985. p. 247–81.
  7. 7. Kairouz S, Gliksman L, Demers A, Adlaf E. For all these reasons, I do …drink: A multilevel analysis of contextual reasons for drinking among Canadian undergraduates. Journal of Studies on Alcohol. 2002;63(5):600–8. pmid:12380857
  8. 8. Fromme K, Dunn M. Alcohol expectancies, social and environmental cues as determinants of drinking and perceived reinforcement. Addictive Behaviors. 1992;17(2):167–77. https://doi.org/10.1016/0306-4603(92)90021-M. pmid:1585843
  9. 9. Giles-Corti B, Donovan R. The relative influence of individual, social and physical environment determinants of physical activity. Social Science and Medicine. 2002;54(12):1793–812. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0277-9536(01)00150-2. pmid:12113436
  10. 10. Teräslinna P, Partanen T, Koskela A, Oja P. Characteristics affecting willingness of executives to participate in an activity program aimed at coronary heart disease prevention. The Journal of Sports Medicine and Physical Fitness. 1969;9(4):224–9. pmid:5377146
  11. 11. Dawson D, Li T-K, Grant B. A prospective study of risk drinking: At risk for what? Drug and Alcohol Dependence. 2008;95(1):62–72.
  12. 12. Wechsler H, Isaac N. 'Binge' drinkers at Massachusetts colleges: Prevalence, drinking style, time trends, and associated problems. Journal of the American Medical Association. 1992;267(21):2929–31. pmid:1583763
  13. 13. Kuntsche E, Gmel G. Alcohol consumption in late adolescence and early adulthood–Where is the problem? Swiss Med Wkly. 2013;143:w13826. pmid:23888405
  14. 14. Parker H, Williams L. Intoxicated weekends: Young adults’ work hard–play hard lifestyles, public health and public disorder. Drugs: Education, Prevention and Policy. 2003;10(4):345–67.
  15. 15. Courtney K, Polich J. Binge drinking in young adults: Data, definitions, and determinants. Psychological bulletin. 2009;135(1):142–56. PubMed PMID: PMC2748736. pmid:19210057
  16. 16. Gmel G, Bissery A, Gammeter R, Givel J, Calmes J, Yersin B, et al. Alcohol‐attributable injuries in admissions to a Swiss emergency room—An analysis of the link between volume of drinking, drinking patterns, and preattendance drinking. Alcoholism: Clinical and Experimental Research. 2006;30(3):501–9.
  17. 17. Zeigler D, Wang C, Yoast R, Dickinson B, McCaffree M, Robinowitz C, et al. The neurocognitive effects of alcohol on adolescents and college students. Preventive Medicine. 2005;40(1):23–32. pmid:15530577
  18. 18. Graham K. Unhappy hours: Alcohol and partner aggression in the Americas: Pan American Health Organization; 2008.
  19. 19. Wechsler H, Lee J, Kuo M, Lee H. College binge drinking in the 1990s: A continuing problem results of the Harvard School of Public Health 1999 College Alcohol Study. Journal of American College Health. 2000;48(5):199–210. pmid:10778020
  20. 20. Wells S, Mihic L, Tremblay P, Graham K, Demers A. Where, with whom, and how much alcohol is consumed on drinking events involving aggression? Event‐level associations in a Canadian national survey of university students. Alcoholism: Clinical and Experimental Research. 2008;32(3):522–33.
  21. 21. Laslett A-M, Room R, Waleewong O, Stanesby O, Callinan S. Harm from others’ drinking: Patterns in nine societies. Geneva, Switzerland: World Health Organization; 2019, in press.
  22. 22. Stanesby O, Callinan S, Graham K, Wilson I, Greenfield T, Wilsnack S, et al. Harm from known others' drinking by relationship proximity to the harmful drinker and gender: A meta-analysis across 10 countries. Alcoholism: Clinical and Experimental Research. 2018;42(9):1693–703. pmid:30035808
  23. 23. Hill K, White H, Chung I, Hawkins D, Catalano R. Early adult outcomes of adolescent binge drinking: Person‐and variable‐centered analyses of binge drinking trajectories. Alcoholism: Clinical and Experimental Research. 2000;24(6):892–901.
  24. 24. Schulenberg J, Maggs J. A developmental perspective on alcohol use and heavy drinking during adolescence and the transition to young adulthood. Journal of Studies on Alcohol. 2002;Supplement(S14):54–70.
  25. 25. Clapp J, Shillington A. Environmental predictors of heavy episodic drinking. The American Journal of Drug and Alcohol Abuse. 2001;27(2):301–13. pmid:11417941
  26. 26. Alexander EN, Bowen AM. Excessive drinking in college: behavioral outcome, not binge, as a basis for prevention. Addict Behav. 2004;29(6):1199–205. pmid:15236823.
  27. 27. Bourdeau B, Miller B, Johnson M, Voas R. Method of transportation and drinking among club patrons. Transp. 2015;32:11–22. pmid:26309425.
  28. 28. Thombs DL, O'Mara R, Dodd VJ, Hou W, Merves ML, Weiler RM, et al. A field study of bar-sponsored drink specials and their associations with patron intoxication. J Stud Alcohol. 2009;70(2):206–14. pmid:19261232.
  29. 29. Foster J, Ferguson C. Alcohol ‘pre-loading’: A review of the literature. Alcohol and Alcoholism. 2013;49(2):213–26. pmid:24049152
  30. 30. Wells S, Graham K, Purcell J. Policy implications of the widespread practice of ‘pre-drinking’ or ‘pre-gaming’ before going to public drinking establishments—Are current prevention strategies backfiring? Addiction. 2009;104(1):4–9. pmid:19133882
  31. 31. Glindemann K, Ehrhart I, Maynard M, Geller S. Alcohol front-loading among college students: Exploring the need for prevention intervention. Journal of Alcohol and Drug Education. 2006;50(2):5–13.
  32. 32. Hughes K, Anderson Z, Morleo M, Bellis M. Alcohol, nightlife and violence: The relative contributions of drinking before and during nights out to negative health and criminal justice outcomes. Addiction. 2008;103(1):60–5. pmid:17996008
  33. 33. Labhart F, Graham K, Wells S, Kuntsche E. Drinking before going to licensed premises: An event-level analysis of predrinking, alcohol consumption, and adverse outcomes. Alcoholism: Clinical and Experimental Research. 2013;37(2):284–91. pmid:23136847.
  34. 34. LaBrie J, Pedersen E. Prepartying promotes heightened risk in the college environment: An event-level report. Addictive Behaviors. 2008;33(7):955–9. Epub 2008/04/05. pmid:18387749; PubMed Central PMCID: PMCPMC3386786.
  35. 35. Kuntsche E, Labhart F. Drinking motives moderate the impact of pre-drinking on heavy drinking on a given evening and related adverse consequences—An event-level study. Addiction. 2013;108(10):1747–55. pmid:23692418.
  36. 36. Thrul J, Kuntsche E. The impact of friends on young adults' drinking over the course of the evening—An event-level analysis. Addiction. 2015;110(4):619–26. pmid:25732756.
  37. 37. Lau-Barraco C, Braitman A, Linden-Carmichael A, Stamates A. Differences in weekday versus weekend drinking among nonstudent emerging adults. Experimental and Clinical Psychopharmacology. 2016;24(2):100–9. pmid:26901592; PubMed Central PMCID: PMCNIHMS761480.
  38. 38. Trim R, Clapp J, Reed M, Shillington A, Thombs D. Drinking plans and drinking outcomes: Examining young adults' weekend drinking behavior. J Drug Educ. 2011;41(3):253–70. pmid:22125921.
  39. 39. Moher D, Liberati A, Tetzlaff J, Altman D, The PRISMA Group. Preferred reporting items for systematic reviews and meta-analyses: The PRISMA statement. PLoS Medicine. 2009;6(7):e1000097. pmid:19621072
  40. 40. Heart National, Lung and Blood Institute. Study quality assessment tools: Quality assessment tool for observational cohort and cross-sectional studies Bethesda, Maryland, United States of America: National Heart, Lung and Blood Institute, National Institutes of Health; 2018 [cited 2018]. Available from: https://www.nhlbi.nih.gov/health-topics/study-quality-assessment-tools.
  41. 41. Sobell L, Sobell M. Timeline follow-back: A technique for assessing self-reported alcohol consumption. In: Litten R, Allen J, editors. Measuring alcohol consumption: Psychosocial and biochemical methods. Totowa, New Jersey, United States and America: Humana Press; 1992. p. 41–72.
  42. 42. Ally A, Lovatt M, Meier P, Brennan A, Holmes J. Developing a social practice-based typology of British drinking culture in 2009–2011: Implications for alcohol policy analysis. Addiction. 2016;111(9):1568–79. pmid:27095617. Language: English. Entry Date: 20160920. Revision Date: 20170901. Publication Type: Article.
  43. 43. Arpin S, Mohr C, Brannan D. Having friends and feeling lonely: A daily process examination of transient loneliness, socialization, and drinking behavior. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin. 2015;41(5):615–28. pmid:25758705
  44. 44. Barnett N, Orchowski L, Read J, Kahler C. Predictors and consequences of pregaming using day- and week-level measurements. Psychol Addict Behav. 2013;27(4):921–33. pmid:23438241; PubMed Central PMCID: PMCNIHMS481733.
  45. 45. Barry A, Stellefson M, Piazza-Gardner A, Chaney B, Dodd V. The impact of pregaming on subsequent blood alcohol concentrations: An event-level analysis. Addictive Behaviors. 2013;38(8):2374–7. pmid:23628431
  46. 46. Bellis M, Hughes K, Quigg Z, Morleo M, Jarman I, Lisboa P. Cross-sectional measures and modelled estimates of blood alcohol levels in UK nightlife and their relationships with drinking behaviours and observed signs of inebriation. Substance Abuse Treatment, Prevention, and Policy. 2010;5:5. pmid:20406433.
  47. 47. Bersamin M, Lipperman-Kreda S, Mair C, Grube J, Gruenewald P. Identifying strategies to limit youth drinking in the home. Journal of Studies on Alcohol and Drugs. 2016;77(6):943–9. pmid:27797696
  48. 48. Bourdeau B, Miller B, Voas R, Johnson M, Byrnes H. Social drinking groups and risk experience in nightclubs: Latent class analysis. Health, Risk and Society. 2017;19(5–6):316–35. pmid:30271266.
  49. 49. Buettner C, Khurana A, Slesnick N. Drinking at college parties: Examining the influence of student host-status and party-location. Addict Behav. 2011;36(12):1365–8. pmid:21862229; PubMed Central PMCID: PMCNIHMS318757.
  50. 50. Carlini C, Andreoni S, Martins S, Benjamin M, Sanudo A, Sanchez Z. Environmental characteristics associated with alcohol intoxication among patrons in Brazilian nightclubs. Drug and Alcohol Review. 2014;33(4):358–66. pmid:24975881.
  51. 51. Cohen S, Lemay E. Why would social networks be linked to affect and health practices? Health Psychol. 2007;26(4):410–7. pmid:17605560.
  52. 52. Clapp J, Lange J, Min J, Shillington A, Johnson M, Voas R. Two studies examining environmental predictors of heavy drinking by college students. Prev Sci. 2003;4(2):99–108. pmid:12751879.
  53. 53. Clapp J, Min J, Shillington A, Reed M, Croff J. Person and environment predictors of blood alcohol concentrations: A multi-level study of college parties. Alcoholism: Clinical and Experimental Research. 2008;32(1):100–7. pmid:18028528.
  54. 54. Dehart T, Tennen H, Armeli S, Todd M, Mohr C. A diary study of implicit self-esteem, interpersonal interactions and alcohol consumption in college students. J Exp Soc Psychol. 2009;45(4):720–30. pmid:20161219.
  55. 55. Dietze P, Agius P, Livingston M, Callinan S, Jenkinson R, Lim MS, et al. Correlates of alcohol consumption on heavy drinking occasions of young risky drinkers: Event versus personal characteristics. Addiction. 2017;112(8):1369–77. pmid:28345281. Language: English. Entry Date: 20170717. Revision Date: 20170724. Publication Type: Article.
  56. 56. Durbeej N, Elgan T, Jalling C, Gripenberg J. Alcohol intoxication at Swedish football matches: A study using biological sampling to assess blood alcohol concentration levels among spectators. PLoS ONE. 2017;12(11):e0188284. pmid:29155885.
  57. 57. Dvorak R, Simons J. Daily associations between anxiety and alcohol use: Variation by sustained attention, set shifting, and gender. Psychol Addict Behav. 2014;28(4):969–79. pmid:25180552.
  58. 58. Fairlie A, Maggs J, Lanza S. Prepartying, drinking games, and extreme drinking among college students: A daily-level investigation. Addict Behav. 2015;42:91–5. pmid:25437263; PubMed Central PMCID: PMCNIHMS645498.
  59. 59. Finlay A, Ram N, Maggs J, Caldwell L. Leisure activities, the social weekend, and alcohol use: Evidence from a daily study of first-year college students. Journal of Studies on Alcohol and Drugs. 2012;73(2):250–9. pmid:22333332.
  60. 60. Groefsema M, Engels R, Kuntsche E, Smit K, Luijten M. Cognitive biases for social alcohol-related pictures and alcohol use in specific social settings: An event-level study. Alcoholism: Clinical and Experimental Research. 2016;40(9):2001–10. pmid:27511292.
  61. 61. Grzywacz J, Almeida D. Stress and binge drinking: A daily process examination of stressor pile-up and socioeconomic status in affect regulation. Int. 2008;15(4):364–80. pmid:19578556.
  62. 62. Harford T. A contextual analysis of drinking events. International Journal of the Addictions. 1983;18(6):825–34. pmid:6629581
  63. 63. Howard A, Patrick M, Maggs J. College student affect and heavy drinking: Variable associations across days, semesters, and people. Psychol Addict Behav. 2015;29(2):430–43. pmid:25347017; PubMed Central PMCID: PMCNIHMS636921.
  64. 64. Hummer J, Napper L, Ehret P, LaBrie J. Event-specific risk and ecological factors associated with prepartying among heavier drinking college students. Addict Behav. 2013;38(3):1620–8. pmid:23254208; PubMed Central PMCID: PMCNIHMS429644.
  65. 65. Jackson K, Colby S, Sher K. Daily patterns of conjoint smoking and drinking in college student smokers. Psychol Addict Behav. 2010;24(3):424–35. pmid:20853927; PubMed Central PMCID: PMCNIHMS205355.
  66. 66. Jih C, Sirgo V, Thomure J. Alcohol consumption, locus of control, and self-esteem of high school and college students. Psychological Reports. 1995;76(3 Pt 1):851–7. pmid:7568601.
  67. 67. Kuntsche E, Otten R, Labhart F. Identifying risky drinking patterns over the course of Saturday evenings: An event-level study. Psychol Addict Behav. 2015;29(3):744–52. pmid:25844829.
  68. 68. Labhart F, Kuntsche E. When yesterday's consumption strikes back: Deviation from usual consumption inversely predicts amounts consumed the next weekend evening. Drug and Alcohol Review. 2014;33(4):385–92. pmid:24802354
  69. 69. Labhart F, Wells S, Graham K, Kuntsche E. Do individual and situational factors explain the link between predrinking and heavier alcohol consumption? An event-level study of types of beverage consumed and social context. Alcohol and Alcoholism. 2014;49(3):327–35. pmid:24481651.
  70. 70. Laws H, Ellerbeck N, Rodrigues A, Simmons J, Ansell E. Social rejection and alcohol use in daily life. Alcoholism: Clinical and Experimental Research. 2017;41(4):820–7. pmid:28253539.
  71. 71. Loxley W, Homel R, Berger D, Snortum J. Drinkers and their driving: Compliance with drinking-driving legislation in four Australian states. Journal of Studies on Alcohol. 1992;53(5):420–6. pmid:1405633.
  72. 72. Luk J, Fairlie A, Lee C. Daily-level associations between negative mood, perceived stress, and college drinking: Do associations differ by sex and fraternity/sorority affiliation? Substance Use and Misuse. 2017:1–9. pmid:29190180.
  73. 73. Luoma J, Guinther P, Lawless DesJardins N, Vilardaga R. Is shame a proximal trigger for drinking? A daily process study with a community sample. Experimental and Clinical Psychopharmacology. 2018;26(3):290–301. pmid:29863385
  74. 74. McClatchley K, Shorter G, Chalmers J. Deconstructing alcohol use on a night out in England: Promotions, preloading and consumption. Drug and Alcohol Review. 2014;33(4):367–75. pmid:24810162.
  75. 75. Meisel M, DiBello A, Balestrieri S, Ott M, DiGuiseppi G, Clark M, et al. An event- and network-level analysis of college students' maximum drinking day. Addict Behav. 2017;79:189–94. pmid:29291510.
  76. 76. Merrill J, Vermont L, Bachrach R, Read J. Is the pregame to blame? Event-level associations between pregaming and alcohol-related consequences. Journal of Studies on Alcohol and Drugs. 2013;74(5):757–64. pmid:23948535.
  77. 77. Mohr C, Armeli S, Tennen H, Carney M, Affleck G, Hromi A. Daily interpersonal experiences, context, and alcohol consumption: Crying in your beer and toasting good times. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 2001;80(3):489–500. pmid:11300581
  78. 78. Mohr C, Armeli S, Tennen H, Temple M, Todd M, Clark J, et al. Moving beyond the keg party: A daily process study of college student drinking motivations. Psychol Addict Behav. 2005;19(4):392–403. pmid:16366811
  79. 79. Mustonen H, Mäkelä P, Lintonen T. Toward a typology of drinking occasions: Latent classes of an autumn week's drinking occasions. Addiction Research and Theory. 2014;22(6):524–34.
  80. 80. O'Grady M, Cullum J, Armeli S, Tennen H. Putting the relationship between social anxiety and alcohol use into context: A daily diary investigation of drinking in response to embarrassing events. Journal of Social and Clinical Psychology. 2011;30(6):599–615. pmid:21874086.
  81. 81. O'Grady M, Cullum J, Tennen H, Armeli S. Daily relationship between event-specific drinking norms and alcohol use: A four-year longitudinal study. Journal of Studies on Alcohol and Drugs. 2011;72(4):633–41. pmid:21683045.
  82. 82. O'Hara R, Armeli S, Tennen H. Drinking-to-cope motivation and negative mood-drinking contingencies in a daily diary study of college students. Journal of Studies on Alcohol and Drugs. 2014;75(4):606–14. pmid:24988259.
  83. 83. Ostergaard J, Skov P. Do pre-drinkers consume more alcohol than non-pre-drinkers on an event-specific night out? A cross-national panel mobile survey of young people's drinking in England and Denmark. Drug and Alcohol Review. 2014;33(4):376–84. pmid:24975998.
  84. 84. Paradis C, Demers A, Nadeau L, Picard E. Parenthood, alcohol intake, and drinking contexts: Occasio furem facit. Journal of Studies on Alcohol and Drugs. 2011;72(2):259–69. pmid:21388599.
  85. 85. Paschall M, Saltz R. Relationships between college settings and student alcohol use before, during and after events: A multi-level study. Drug and Alcohol Review. 2007;26(6):635–44. pmid:17943524.
  86. 86. Patrick M, Yeomans-Maldonado G, Griffin J. Daily reports of positive and negative affect and alcohol and marijuana use among college student and nonstudent young adults. Substance Use and Misuse. 2016;51(1):54–61. pmid:26683453; PubMed Central PMCID: PMCNIHMS769625 [Available on 01/02/17].
  87. 87. Peacock A, Norman T, Bruno R, Pennay A, Droste N, Jenkinson R, et al. Typology of alcohol consumers in five Australian nighttime entertainment districts. Drug and Alcohol Review. 2016;35(5):539–48. pmid:26661588.
  88. 88. Pedersen E, Labrie J. Partying before the party: Examining prepartying behavior among college students. J Am Coll Health. 2007;56(3):237–45. pmid:18089504; PubMed Central PMCID: PMCNIHMS63577.
  89. 89. Pennay A, Miller P, Busija L, Jenkinson R, Droste N, Quinn B, et al. 'Wide-awake drunkenness'? Investigating the association between alcohol intoxication and stimulant use in the night-time economy. Addiction. 2015;110(2):356–65. pmid:25220360.
  90. 90. Quigg Z, Hughes K, Bellis M. Student drinking patterns and blood alcohol concentration on commercially organised pub crawls in the UK. Addict Behav. 2013;38(12):2924–9. pmid:24064191.
  91. 91. Santos M, Paes A, Sanudo A, Sanchez Z. Factors associated with pre-drinking among nightclub patrons in the city of Sao Paulo. Alcohol and Alcoholism. 2015;50(1):95–102. pmid:25164117.
  92. 92. Simons J, Gaher R, Oliver M, Bush J, Palmer M. An experience sampling study of associations between affect and alcohol use and problems among college students. Journal of Studies on Alcohol. 2005;66(4):459–69. pmid:16240553.
  93. 93. Simons J, Dvorak R, Batien B, Wray T. Event-level associations between affect, alcohol intoxication, and acute dependence symptoms: Effects of urgency, self-control, and drinking experience. Addict Behav. 2010;35(12):1045–53. pmid:20685044; PubMed Central PMCID: PMCNIHMS227342.
  94. 94. Smit K, Groefsema M, Luijten M, Engels R, Kuntsche E. Drinking motives moderate the effect of the social environment on alcohol use: An event-level study among young adults. Journal of Studies on Alcohol and Drugs. 2015;76(6):971–80. pmid:26562607.
  95. 95. Stappenbeck C, Hassija C, Zimmerman L, Kaysen D. Sexual assault related distress and drinking: The influence of daily reports of social support and coping control. Addict Behav. 2015;42:108–13. pmid:25437266; PubMed Central PMCID: PMCNIHMS727151.
  96. 96. Thrul J, Kuntsche E. Interactions between drinking motives and friends in predicting young adults' alcohol use. Prev Sci. 2016;17(5):626–35. pmid:27165112; PubMed Central PMCID: PMCNIHMS787409 [Available on 07/01/17].
  97. 97. Tutenges S, Jaeger M, Hesse M. The influence of guides on alcohol consumption among young tourists at a nightlife resort. Am J Addict. 2012;21(Supplement 1):S72–S6. pmid:23786514.
  98. 98. Wells S, Dumas T, Bernards S, Kuntsche E, Labhart F, Graham K. Predrinking, alcohol use, and breath alcohol concentration: A study of young adult bargoers. Psychol Addict Behav. 2015;29(3):683–9. pmid:25984585.
  99. 99. Smyth J, Stone A. Ecological momentary assessment research in behavioral medicine. Journal of Happiness Studies. 2003;4(1):35–52.
  100. 100. Lovatt M, Holmes J. Digital phenotyping and sociological perspectives in a Brave New World. Addiction. 2017;112(7):1286–9. pmid:28472847
  101. 101. Santani D, Do T, Labhart F, Landolt S, Kuntsche E, Gatica-Perez D. DrinkSense: Characterizing youth drinking behavior using smartphones. IEEE Transactions on Mobile Computing. 2018;17(10):2279–92.
  102. 102. Kuntsche E, Gatica-Perez D, Landolt S. Youth@Night: The project 2018 [cited 2018]. Available from: http://www.youth-night.ch/. Accessed 21 August 2018. Archived at http://www.webcitation.org/71pKMb688.
  103. 103. Skinner A, Attwood A, Baddeley R, Evans‐Reeves K, Bauld L, Munafò M. Digital phenotyping and the development and delivery of health guidelines and behaviour change interventions. Addiction. 2017;112(7):1281–5. pmid:28472848
  104. 104. Enders C, Tofighi D. Centering predictor variables in cross-sectional multilevel models: A new look at an old issue. Psychological Methods. 2007;12(2):121–38. pmid:17563168
  105. 105. Hoffman L, Stawski R. Persons as contexts: Evaluating between-person and within-person effects in longitudinal analysis. Research in Human Development. 2009;6(2–3):97–120.
  106. 106. Coughlin S. Recall bias in epidemiologic studies. Journal of Clinical Epidemiology. 1990;43(1):87–91. pmid:2319285
  107. 107. Leffingwell T, Cooney N, Murphy J, Luczak S, Rosen G, Dougherty D, et al. Continuous objective monitoring of alcohol use: Twenty‐first century measurement using transdermal sensors. Alcoholism: Clinical and Experimental Research. 2013;37(1):16–22.
  108. 108. Collins L, Kashdan T, Gollnisch G. The feasibility of using cellular phones to collect ecological momentary assessment data: Application to alcohol consumption. Experimental and Clinical Psychopharmacology. 2003;11(1):73–8. pmid:12622345
  109. 109. Kuntsche E, Rehm J, Gmel G. Characteristics of binge drinkers in Europe. Social Science and Medicine. 2004;59(1):113–27. pmid:15087148
  110. 110. Naimi T, Brewer R, Mokdad A, Denny C, Serdula M, Marks J. Binge drinking among US adults. Journal of the American Medical Association. 2003;289(1):70–5. pmid:12503979
  111. 111. Pennay A, Livingston M, MacLean S. Young people are drinking less: It is time to find out why. Drug and Alcohol Review. 2015;34(2):115–8. pmid:25756218
  112. 112. Livingston M, Callinan S, Dietze P, Stanesby O, Kuntsche E. Is there gender convergence in risky drinking when taking birth cohorts into account? Evidence from an Australian national survey 2001‐13. Addiction. 2018;113(11):2019–28. Epub 2018/05/29. pmid:29806883
  113. 113. Livingston M. Understanding recent trends in Australian alcohol consumption. Canberra: Foundation for Alcohol Research and Education, 2015.
  114. 114. Raninen J, Leifman H, Ramstedt M. Who Is not drinking less in Sweden? An analysis of the decline in consumption for the period 2004–2011. Alcohol and Alcoholism. 2013;48(5):592–7. pmid:23729672