Skip to main content
Advertisement
Browse Subject Areas
?

Click through the PLOS taxonomy to find articles in your field.

For more information about PLOS Subject Areas, click here.

  • Loading metrics

Beauty, elegance, grace, and sexiness compared

Abstract

Beauty is the single most frequently and most broadly used aesthetic virtue term. The present study aimed at providing higher conceptual resolution to the broader notion of beauty by comparing it with three closely related aesthetically evaluative concepts which are likewise lexicalized across many languages: elegance, grace(fulness), and sexiness. We administered a variety of questionnaires that targeted perceptual qualia, cognitive and affective evaluations, as well as specific object properties that are associated with beauty, elegance, grace, and sexiness in personal looks, movements, objects of design, and other domains. This allowed us to reveal distinct and highly nuanced profiles of how a beautiful, elegant, graceful, and sexy appearance is subjectively perceived. As aesthetics is all about nuances, the fine-grained conceptual analysis of the four target concepts of our study provides crucial distinctions for future research.

Introduction

Both in lay concepts of aesthetic perception and evaluation [13] and in empirical research, beauty is by far the single most important aesthetic virtue term. To be sure, already 18th century aesthetics knew of several other target virtues, such as the sublime ([46]; see also [79]) and the interesting ([10,11]; see also [1214]). Today, the overall picture is far more diversified (for a survey of several dozens of relevant terms, see [2] and [15]). Still, beauty ratings remain the prime currency of empirical studies in aesthetics. Supporting this tendency, theoretical accounts of aesthetic appeal and concomitant pleasure and liking––such as the processing fluency hypothesis cf. [1621]––also show a bias towards explaining attributions of beauty rather than of the sublime or of other aesthetic virtues.

The present article does not challenge this primacy of beauty. Rather, it is aimed at an inner diversification of the beauty pole of aesthetics. It is distinctive of the concept of beauty that is used in a very broad way, at least so in the Western tradition since Greek antiquity. On the one hand, it encompasses the sexual beauty of bodily looks and natural beauty in general (landscapes, animals, etc.); on the other hand, beauty is also attributed to a great variety of cultural artefacts, including classical artworks and numerous objects of design. In the present study, we focus on three concepts each of which we expected to show great semantic overlap with the broader concept of beauty, while at the same time distinctly capturing special facets of beauty on the nature-culture continuum.

  1. For the cultural beauty pole, we selected “elegance.” In Latin, “elegantia” meant refinement, tasteful correctness, and decorousness, with a specific focus on carefully choosing––lat. eligere, to elect––one’s words [22]. Starting in the 14th century, the concept has increasingly come to be applied to multiple domains, including refined personal looks, many objects of design (evening wear, jewelry, hairstyles, furniture, cars––specifically, cabriolets and limousines––, yachts, villas, restaurants, bridges, skyscrapers, etc.), movements, verbal articulation and argumentation, and to unexpectedly smooth solutions to difficult cognitive or technical problems (see S1 Text). Compared to beauty, elegance is far less frequently and far more selectively applied to natural objects, with tigers, leopards and other cats being among the exceptions (not least by virtue of their elegance in movement).
  2. For the natural (sexual) beauty pole, we selected the modern term “sexy.” To be sure, this term is not exclusively applied to natural bodily looks, but also to clothing and self-styling and hence to cultural ways of enhancing or highlighting physical sexual attractiveness. The term “sexy” has no tradition in modern philosophical aesthetics, as many, though by no means all authors (see, for instance, Edmund Burke, [4], p. 115) programmatically tried to purge aesthetics and even the theory of beauty of all reminders of sexual desire and biological functions. At the same time, a sexual dimension of beauty attributions to individuals plays an important role in the cultural record from antiquity through modernity, and Charles Darwin’s account of competitive beauty displays throughout nature and human cultures lends much credit to this rich tradition [23].
  3. As a third term closely related to beauty, we included the concept of “grace.” Schiller defined grace (German Anmut) as “beauty in motion” ([24]; see also Burke [4], p. 119 and [25]), whereas Lord Kames defined it as “elegance in movement” ([26], p. 364). Today, too, grace is nearly exclusively attributed to the movements of people (and some animals), but barely ever to inanimate objects. Regarding the nature-culture-continuum of beauty, grace, for all its overlap with elegance and hence cultural sophistication, appears to lean more towards the natural pole in that graceful movements can well be found in young children and animals and hence are less dependent on cultural sophistication than typical forms of elegance in movement, such as in ballet and other forms of dance. Still, in accordance with the classical definitions, we expected that grace should show far greater overlap with elegance and beauty than with sexiness.

Remarkably, across all major Germanic, Romance and Slavic languages, the Finno-Ugric languages, many Asian languages (Chinese, Japanese, Korean), and potentially beyond, words for “elegance” are lexicalized in phonologically convergent form, i.e., based on the Latin word “elegantia.” For “grace” and “sexy,” a similar adoption of the Latin words “gratia” and “sexus” applies across many languages, albeit not to the same degree. The successful migration of these terms into many other languages suggests that they capture special facets of aesthetic appeal that are salient, in however culturally varying form, across centuries and cultures.

Our study is the first to comparatively analyze the four categories under scrutiny––beauty, elegance, grace, and sexiness––for their distinct perceptual, cognitive and affective implications that together make up their meaning. Specifically, it is aimed at elucidating, with high granularity, the mental constructs which individuals have acquired regarding beauty, elegance, grace, and sexiness. Linguistic concepts are reflective of mental constructs largely shared across linguistic communities, in some cases also across different languages and cultures. Our study tapped into these mental constructs using a variety of methods, including free associations, questionnaires and semantic differentials. We did not present any beautiful, elegant, graceful, and/or sexy stimuli in rating tasks. Rather, we exclusively targeted the very constructs that guide individuals when they end up perceiving and categorizing specific phenomena as beautiful, elegant, graceful, or sexy.

The two studies

For efforts aimed at contrasting hypothetical sub-types of a broader class of phenomena, the general category is the unmarked class. A focus on bringing out differences between our four categories may therefore be better served by placing a primary focus on one of the more circumscribed categories. Following this reasoning, our first study was primarily devoted to investigating characteristics of elegance. It opens its scope to also include beauty and sexiness only in the part which addresses the factor of age in attributions of beautiful, elegant and sexy personal looks.

In the absence of a full-blown theory of elegance––in fact, of any theory of elegance––, we resorted to well-established methods of investigating uncharted phenomena and concepts, such as free associations and semantic differentials (for all details, see the Method sections of Studies 1 and 2). We designed a variety of open-format questions as well as scales and questionnaires aimed at elucidating which cognitive, emotional, and social characteristics are associated with elegant individuals and which features of movement and verbal performance are considered elegant. In this context, we also collected the very first data sets that directly compare perceived beauty, elegance, and sexiness in personal appearance.

Study 2 extended this comparison to a broad range of cognitive, affective, and social implications associated with all four target categories of our study. Informed by the primarily elegance-related results of Study 1 as well as by theoretical anticipations, we collected, in a between-subject design, ratings for beauty, elegance, grace, and sexiness, respectively, on 42 semantic differentials (i.e., polar adjective pairs). Osgood and colleagues [27] introduced semantic differentials as a method to analyze with high conceptual resolution even minor differences of target categories on subjectively perceived qualitative dimensions. We adopted this method for our purposes.

Study 1

The first part of Study 1 consisted of a free association task: participants were asked to write down words that they associate with elegance. Association tasks of this type have been shown to provide access to conceptual affinities informing the meaning of a specific concept [28,29]. The subsequent blocks of questions targeted person-related characteristics dependent on an overall elegant personal appearance and descriptors of elegant physical build and gait. As elegant diction is historically the classical domain of elegance, we also investigated to what extent contemporary individuals have a clear understanding of this type of elegance, too. To this end, we presented them with attributes that are often associated with verbal elegance.

Going beyond elegance only, we also asked our participants to indicate whether or not it is likely in their view that women and men, respectively, are perceived as beautiful, elegant, and/or sexy in predefined age ranges.

Methods

Participants.

One hundred fifty individuals participated in this study (115 female, 34 male, 1 undisclosed; mean age in years M = 30.1, SD = 14.4, median = 24, min = 18, max = 77; for a histogram of the age distribution, see S2 Text). They were mostly recruited via the general study participant database of the Max Planck Institute for Empirical Aesthetics (which by the time the study was performed comprised some 1.500 participants covering a broad range in age, educational profiles, professions, etc.).

Most of the participants (77%) reported a monthly income of up to 1,500 EUR. 34% held a university degree or similar qualification; of the remaining participants, 56% held a secondary school diploma (Abitur). The age distribution and the data on the highest degree of education together indicate that the greater part of the sample had attended or were currently attending university.

One hundred forty-six participants were native speakers of German, 20 of them bilingual. We report the results for all 150 participants, as excluding the four non-native speakers did not change the results. (For further details on the sample, see S2 Text.)

Ethics statement.

Study 1 was conducted in full accordance with the World Medical Association’s Declaration of Helsinki and the Ethical Guidelines of the German Association of Psychologists (DGPs). All participants gave their written informed consent and received EUR 5 as compensation for their participation. All procedures of the study were approved by the Ethics Council of the Max Planck Society.

Questionnaire.

The questionnaire (which is available at the Open Science Framework, https://osf.io/uqga9/) was presented on a computer in a lab room. In the free association task, participants were asked to write down between one and twenty words they associate with elegance.

Subsequently, they were asked to indicate on 7-point Likert scales how strongly they associate elegant individuals with several cognitive, emotional, and social characteristics, and to what degree they consider specific features as distinctive of elegance in gait and diction. Again, in the absence of available scales, we selected our rating items in a lengthy process that was informed by our intuitive understanding of elegance, lexicon entries, and discussion among the authors. We also consulted research on personality attributions dependent on physical attractiveness [3032].

Additionally, we collected data on the physical build, height, and leg length participants considered prototypical for an elegant personal appearance. For physical build, participants could indicate whether attributes such as “slender,” “corpulent”, “athletic,” etc. are compatible with an elegant appearance. For both height and leg length, participants could choose between three degrees (tall/long, medium, small/short).

In a next step, we asked our participants to indicate categorically for each of seven predefined age ranges whether or not they consider it likely that women and men, respectively, are perceived as beautiful, elegant, and/or sexy in these age ranges. We limited the lowest age range to the four years from age 16 to 19 in order to rule out that kids younger than sixteen years of age would need to be projected onto the mental template of sexual appeal. Above this age, we collected data for all decades from age 20 to 90. We did not ask how great a percentage of men and women of these age ranges they perceive as beautiful, elegant, and/or sexy, because we expected that this would be a fairly demanding question that could prevent our participants from making a quick, spontaneous choice. Still, as participants responded to the same task for all age ranges, we expected that the data should well be informative regarding relative differences in subjective perceptions of beautiful, elegant, and sexy personal looks dependent on age.

Furthermore, participants were asked (1) to list public figures they find elegant, (2) to rate several experimenter-selected classes of objects regarding a) how frequently individual items in these classes are elegant, and b) the degree of elegance these objects can reach in their view, and (3) indicate their own personal habits and preferences regarding elegant clothing. The results regarding these questions are reported in the Supporting Information (see S4 and S5 Tables and S2 Text).

Results and discussion

The data and analysis scripts for Study 1 and 2 are available at Open Science Framework (https://osf.io/uqga9/).

Free association data.

On average, participants wrote down 11.3 associations (median = 10, SD = 4.96, min = 2, max = 20, with 24 participants producing the allowed maximum of 20 associations). We preprocessed the data by correcting spelling mistakes and by subsuming all nouns and adjectives of the same word stem (such as “beauty” and “beautiful”) under one category only. All in all, 1,699 associations and 816 words (637 after categorizing) were generated.

For the following analysis, we retained only the 43 words that were mentioned by at least 5% of the participants. Frequencies, mean list rank, and the Cognitive Salience index for these 43 predominant associations are reported in Table 1. The Cognitive Salience index reflects the absolute Frequency of the terms mentioned (= F) divided by the product of the Number of participants (= N) and the Mean List Rank of the terms (MLR): CSI = F/(N * MLR). The CSI is bounded between 0 and 1, with higher values reflecting higher salience of words for a conceptual domain [33].

thumbnail
Table 1. Words most frequently associated with elegance, ranked by their cognitive salience index (CSI).

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0218728.t001

Highlighting the close affinity of three of our target concepts, participants associated elegance most frequently and most saliently with beauty and grace. In contrast, our fourth target concept, sexy, was not listed at all. Aesthetics, style, svelte, clothing, and fashion followed next. Exploratory analyses of the data revealed a fine-grained, complex network of associations (see S3 Text and S1, S2 and S3 Figs). As these analyses yielded no clear-cut clusters or dimensions, we here base our discussion on semantic grounds. We distinguish four groups of word associations:

(1) General perceptual qualities and aesthetic appeal dimensions of elegance.

Entries include impressions of lightness/delicacy, fluency (supple, fluent), harmony, calm and exquisiteness (glamour/splendor, finesse, classic, charisma). For all these extraordinary virtues, elegance is still associated with simplicity. Three of these major characteristics of elegance––namely, fluency, lightness, and simplicity––represent key concepts, or synonyms thereof, of the processing fluency or ease of processing hypothesis of aesthetic liking [18,3437]. Elegance defies both physical gravity and intellectual difficulty: despite their enormous physical weight, elegant bridges spanning over miles can appear light, and elegant solutions of very difficult intellectual problems similarly come with an air of simplicity and lightness. Importantly, it is not just cognitive fluency, but, more specifically, cognitive fluency combined with (a) pronounced impressions of perceptual lightness, (b) blends of refinement and highly artful simplicity, and (c) a strong factor of exquisiteness/preciousness that renders elegance a highly distinct variant of fluency-supported aesthetic appeal.

(2) Elegance in personal appearance.

Entries focus particularly on fashion/clothing (comprised of the listings for fashion, dress, evening gown, clothing, and suit), physical movement and posture (dance, movement, gait, posture), and personal looks (svelte, looks, chic). Even though men's suits are also listed, the data show a far stronger association of elegance with female (the entries being woman, lady, and femininity) than with male looks.

(3) Social selectivity and high socio-economic standing associated with elegance.

Relevant entries include the words distinguished, expensive, wealth, und glamour (see S2 Fig, especially the upper part).

(4) Elegance in the arts.

Even though art-related associations were listed far less frequently than those for personal elegance, the concept of elegance was associated with artistic achievements, too. However, only dance, art, ballet, music, and poetry (in descending order) passed the 5% cut-off we adopted for our analysis of the data.

Physical build and gait of elegant individuals.

Prototypical physical features regarding elegant women are, in descending order, slender, delicate, thin, and at least mean height; for elegant men, athletic, slender, and tall are the most frequently expected features. Only between 0.0% and 6.7% percent of the participants associated an elegant physical build also (but by no means exclusively) with the experimenter-selected attributes corpulent, portly, plump and stocky. Ratings for delicate and athletic show the most pronounced dissociation between female and male elegance (see S4 Fig). The data for an elegant physical build of men are associated with tall body height and medium leg length, whereas those for an elegant physical build of women show, inversely, a stronger association with medium body height and long legs (see S5 Fig). Hence slenderness and long legs, while prototypical expectations for an elegant physical build in both women and men, appear to play an even more pronounced role for female than for male elegance.

Regarding the elegance of gait (for which we did not collect separate data sets for women and men), the characteristics upright carriage, fluent, and light were the most important, followed by dancerly and elastic (for all details see S1 Table).

Non-physical person variables associated with an elegant appearance.

The elegance-dependent attributions we obtained for non-physical person variables include cognitive and verbal capacities (intelligent, eloquent, creative, witty), acquired cultural capital (cultured, educated, self-confident; for the notion of “cultural capital”, see [38]), prosocial and moral characteristics (correct, sociable, conscientious, responsible, honest, caring), social habitus (polite, charming, gallant, open) and social/professional success (successful, rich) (for all details see S2 Table).

These elegance-driven inferences can be considered an analogue to the physical attractiveness stereotype, i.e., to the attribution of several personality traits dependent on physical attractiveness [3032]. Of course, such attributions are likely to be misattributions in many cases, as is true of all stereotypes. However, the goal of the present study was precisely to identify prototypical elegance-driven attributions not only of physical, but also of non-physical person variables. (A systematic comparison of the associations found for physical attractiveness and elegance was beyond the goals of our study.)

Age-dependency of sexy, beautiful, and elegant personal looks.

The next target variable of our study was the factor of age. As explained in the Method section, the task was expected to yield information about relative differences in our participants’ spontaneous attributions of beauty, elegance, and sexiness (or absence thereof) to personal looks dependent on age, but not about absolute frequencies of such looks. Fig 1 shows the responses per age range and attribute (beauty, elegant, sexy) for men and women, respectively.

thumbnail
Fig 1. Subjectively perceived age-dependency of sexy, beautiful, and elegant personal looks.

Bars represent the number of participants indicating that in the respective age ranges women (dark grey) and men (light grey) are likely to be perceived as elegant (lower part), beautiful (middle part), and sexy (upper part). Multiple entries for age range per concept and gender were possible.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0218728.g001

Logistic mixed-effects regression models predicted probabilities of responses in a full factorial design while accounting for non-independence of observations within participants due to multiple responses (the 90+ age category was omitted due to an empty cell). Probabilities of responses differed between attributes, age ranges, and gender (three-way interaction, χ²(14) = 39.4, p = .0003). Sidak-corrected post-hoc comparisons showed that significant gender differences were observed for sexiness attributions at some age ranges, with higher female sexiness at age 16–19 (p < .0001) and higher male sexiness at age 30–39 (p = .010), 40–49 (p < .0001), and 50–59 (p < .0001), but not for elegance (ps > .23) and beauty attributions (ps > .15) at any age.

At the same time, the data for sexiness and beauty attributions dependent on age-ranges are descriptively far more similar to one another than they are to those for elegance attributions. The latter reach peak levels only far later in age, with the ages 50 to 59 representing the very peak (contrast to sexiness: p < .0001; to beauty: p < .0001), and fairly high chances of being considered elegant extending even into old age. In contrast, elegance attributed to youths of 16–19 years is negligible (contrast to sexiness: p < .0001; to beauty: p < .0001), and remains significantly lower compared to attributions of beauty and sexiness (all ps ≤ .005) for the subsequent two decades. For the age range of 40–49, attributions of beauty and elegance show no significant difference anymore (p = .51), and sexiness attributions are significantly lower than those for elegance (p < .0001). Across all remaining age ranges, elegance attributions are significantly higher than those for sexiness and beauty (ps < .0001).

Thus, our data show a nearly perfect dissociation of elegant and sexy looks at both young and older ages. For beauty and elegance, the pattern of dissociation was structurally similar, but far less pronounced. Chances of being considered elegant at age 60 and older are roughly two times as high as chances of being considered beautiful and four times as high as chances of being considered sexy. Hence, in our data, sexiness and elegance of personal appearance largely diverge, with beauty representing a middle ground that show roughly the same amount of overlap with elegant and with sexy looks, respectively.

Whereas the parameters of elegant bodily looks (slenderness, long legs, at least mean height) largely converge with current (Western) ideals of physical attractiveness (beauty), elegance in personal appearance strongly deviates from the current idealization of beautiful youthful looks in one important respect: it is about culturally refined good looks not only of mature adults, but also of individuals in their advanced ages and even in very old age.

Overall, results show a pronounced nature-culture difference across the three categories. The “sexy”-attributions for women reach earlier high levels than those for men, are equal for both sexes at the age 20–29, and then decline for both sexes, albeit far more rapidly and drastically so for women than for men. This distribution is likely to reflect differences in the age-ranges in which women and men, respectively, are biologically most likely to have off-spring and can hence be considered sexually “attractive” in a narrower meaning.

It is all the more remarkable, then, how strongly beauty attributions depart from these biological constraints. The decline in beauty attributions is far less steep and swift than in sexiness attributions. Perhaps even more importantly, this decline does not show any larger differences for women and men across all age ranges. This supports the assumption that beauty attributions to persons are clearly and markedly different from attributions of sexual attractiveness, even though they are likely to also entail aspects of natural physical beauty.

Regarding elegance, the difference to sexiness attributions is even more pronounced. The data for the likelihood of elegance attributions show even far higher absolute levels than those for beauty until very high age, and significant gender differences are not found in our data from age 30 onward. In this sense, elegance appears to the most cultural and the most “age-fair” type of beauty.

Thus, as expected, sexiness and elegance attributions show the greatest dissociation, with beauty attributions covering the middle ground.

Elegance of verbal articulation and argumentation.

The ratings obtained for elegant diction are high (in descending order) for well-spoken, poised, meticulously articulated, nuanced wording, rich in vocabulary, exquisite vocabulary, eloquent, precise in expression, sophisticated, correct, pointed, quick-witted, rich in imagery, and playful, and low for complicated, verbose, stilted, long-winded, and monotonous (for all details see S3 Table). Several of the high-ranking items of our questionnaire specifically apply to contexts of conversation and oral performance, particularly quick-witted, meticulously articulated, and partly also poised. Overall, the results indicate a nuanced persistence of the older notion of rhetorical elegance among contemporary individuals.

All data obtained in Study 1 were furthermore separately analyzed for differences dependent on the participants’ age and gender. Overall, the understanding of elegance was found to be very consistent and only marginally affected by these variables (for all details, see S4 Text).

Study 2

Whereas the comparison of beauty, elegance and sexiness in Study 1 was limited to dimensions of personal appearance, Study 2 proceeded to a full-blown comparison of beauty, elegance, grace, and sexiness regarding which perceptual, affective, cognitive, and also social dimensions/associations are in general held to be distinctive of the four concepts. The comparison was performed using semantic differentials [27]. Participants were asked to characterize their understanding of beauty, elegance, grace(fulness), and sexiness, respectively, through ratings on 42 items presented on 7-point scales. The opposite ends of the 42 scales were antithetical adjectives.

Methods

Participants.

Four hundred forty-six students with a mean age of 23.1 years (SD = 7.32, median = 21, min = 16, max = 75) participated in Study 2 (262 female, 182 male, 2 undisclosed). Three hundred seventy-one were native speakers of German; 67 were bilingual, 7 non-native speakers, and 1 undisclosed. Each participant gave ratings for one of the target concepts only. We excluded the data (a) of the 1 undisclosed and the 7 non-native-speaking participants and (b) of 8 participants who completed less than 50% of the ratings. Thus, we ended up with 430 valid datasets (108 for beauty, 111 for elegance, 102 for grace, and 109 for sexiness).

Ethics statement.

Study 2 was conducted in full accordance with the World Medical Association’s Declaration of Helsinki and the Ethical Guidelines of the German Association of Psychologists (DGPs). Participation was based on implicit rather than explicit informed consent; non-consenting individuals did not produce any data or did not return a questionnaire. We did not record this type of consent, in accordance with the rules for dispensing with recording/documenting informed consent that are stipulated in § C.III.6 of the Ethical Guidelines of the German Association of Psychologists [39]. (Note that for the completely anonymous data acquisition we performed, it is not only not mandatory, but also unusual at German universities to ask for written informed consent.)

Questionnaire.

Based on intuitive and theory-based anticipations, co-occurrences in linguistic corpora, and the free associations of Study 1, we first collected 145 pairs of adjectives as candidate terms. The authors and colleagues (n = 6) rated these pairs with regard to the degree to which they might reveal differences between the concepts of beauty, elegance, grace, and sexiness. By selecting only those pairs that had a high mean and a low standard deviation in the ratings, we obtained 39 of the 42 adjective pairs listed in Fig 2. Three additional pairs were selected for theoretical reasons: “pleasant–unpleasant” as a classical marker of valence, “natural–artificial” because the nature-art distinction is of major importance in aesthetics in general, and “monochrome–multi-colored” because we wanted to check whether the very reduced color spectrum associated with elegance in the free association task in Study 1––only the entry “black” passed the 5% cut-off––could be replicated using a different method. (See at the Open Science Framework, https://osf.io/uqga9/, for the German and an English version of the questionnaire.)

thumbnail
Fig 2. Semantic differential profiles for beauty, elegance, grace, and sexiness.

Mean values plus 95% CI band for the concepts beauty (blue), elegance (black), gracefulness (green), and sexiness (red).

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0218728.g002

Procedure.

The data were collected at the end of several lecture classes (computer science, literary studies, biology, physics, sports, psychology, cultural anthropology) at Goethe University Frankfurt, Germany. Students were asked if they would volunteer to participate in the study, which only took a few minutes. They were explicitly informed about the task they were expected to perform (i.e., to rate, within two minutes, the concept of either beauty, grace, elegance, or sexiness on various scales, and to also indicate their gender, age, field of study, and native language), the anonymity of the data, the fully voluntary nature of their participation, and their right to withdraw from the study at any time. We handed out the questionnaire consisting of one sheet of paper only to those who volunteered and retrieved it after two minutes. No identifying personal information was obtained.

Results and discussion

We report analyses of the overlap and differences between all four categories on three levels of resolution. First, we looked at overall differences by calculating the mean absolute differences based on all ratings obtained for all semantic differentials as well as calculating MANOVAs for each pair of concepts. Second, we performed an exploratory factor analysis and calculated for each pair of concepts separate MANOVAs for the items making up the seven individual factors. Finally, we identified the respective individual differentials on which each target concept converges with, and diverges from, the other target concepts.

Mean absolute differences between the target categories across all semantic differentials.

The overall mean of the absolute differences across all 42 semantic differentials measured on 7-point scales are 0.442 (SD = 0.288) for beauty and elegance, 0.328 (SD = 0.243) for beauty and grace, 0.361 (SD = 0.349) for beauty and sexiness, 0.180 (SD = 0.130) for elegance and grace, 0.649 (SD = 0.417) for elegance and sexiness, and 0.562 (SD = 0.425) for grace and sexiness. Highlighting the role of beauty as a middle ground, the overall mean of the absolute differences between beauty and the other three categories are all within a narrow margin (ranging from 0.328 to 0.442). Comparing elegance and grace with sexiness yielded the highest mean differences (0.649 and 0.562, respectively), whereas elegance and grace show a particularly low mean of the absolute differences of only 0.180, and hence are barely distinguishable on our 42-item measure.

MANOVAs performed for pairwise comparisons of all target categories across all semantic differentials yielded a convergent picture. The highest significant difference was obtained for elegance and sexiness (Wilk’s λ = 0.310, Pillai’s trace = 0.690, F(42,109) = 5.78, p < .001), followed by the difference of grace and sexiness (Wilk’s λ = 0.316, Pillai’s trace = 0.684, F(42,105) = 5.41, p < .001). The lowest difference was again found for elegance and grace (Wilk’s λ = 0.714, Pillai’s trace = 0.286, F(42,109) = 1.04, p = .42) and did not reach significance. The other three comparisons yielded levels of significant differences that cover the middle ground between these extremes: beauty and elegance (Wilk’s λ = 0.460, Pillai’s trace = 0.540, F(42,110) = 3.08, p < .001), beauty and sexiness (Wilk’s λ = 0.498, Pillai’s trace = 0.502, F(42,106) = 2.55, p < .001), and beauty and grace (Wilk’s λ = 0.533, Pillai’s trace = 0.467, F(42,106) = 2.21, p < .001).

Factor analysis across all four data sets.

We performed a post-hoc exploratory factor analysis across all four data sets (beauty, elegance, grace, and sexiness) using the R psych package [40]. The overall KMO index was .85, indicating suitability for factor analysis [41]. Parallel analysis [42,43] yielded seven factors to be extracted. We extracted these seven factors using principal axis method with oblique rotation. The factors can be interpreted as (1) Tastefulness/Fluency, (2) Affective Habitus/Arousal, (3) Cultural Sophistication, (4) Intellectual Rigor, (5) Lightness/Slenderness, (6) Exquisiteness, and (7) Delicacy/Femininity (see S6 Table, for loadings of the 42 items on the seven factors).

In accordance with the findings of the analyses of the mean absolute differences, MANOVAs (Bonferroni-corrected for multiple testing; see Table 2 for a summary and S7 Table for statistical details) revealed that elegance differs significantly from grace on none of the seven factors, from sexiness on all factors, and from beauty on an intermediate number (four) of factors (Tastefulness/Fluency, Intellectual Rigor, Lightness/Slenderness, and Exquisiteness). In contrast, beauty differs from sexiness only on the two factors Affective Habitus/Arousal and Delicacy/Femininity. Grace and sexiness are significantly different on all factors except factor 3 (Cultural Sophistication).

thumbnail
Table 2. Summary results for the pairwise comparison of the four concepts for each of the seven factors.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0218728.t002

Differences between the four target categories dependent on individual semantic differentials.

Finally, we compared all four categories (beauty, elegance, grace, and sexiness) pairwise for differences and convergences on the level of all 42 semantic differentials. We did so by computing Bayes factors (as alternative to the conventional t-test) [4446] for each adjective pair using the ttestBF function of the R package BayesFactor [47]. The default settings allowed conclusions regarding evidence for significant differences, absence thereof (no difference), and inconclusive results (for all details, see S5 Text). We here highlight the most important findings regarding positive evidence for differences and absence of differences.

Looking at individual items, beauty as compared to elegance is conceived as being more multi-colored, natural, and dreamy, and less refined, rigorous, elitist, disciplined, exquisite, expensive, exceptional, rich, svelte, and skillful. Beauty as compared to sexiness scores higher on the items soothing, delicate, fragile, feminine, simple, reserved, quiet, refined and discreet, and lower on the items libidinous, extroverted and hot. Beauty as compared to grace is conceived as more multi-colored, natural, down to earth, younger, and hotter, and as less elitist, rich, skillful, quiet, exquisite, disciplined, refined and exceptional. Given the close affinity of elegance and grace, it is not surprising––and highlights the consistent quality of our data––that the differences obtained for beauty and grace largely overlap with those for beauty and elegance.

Elegance compared to sexiness is conceived as less arousing, less extroverted, less libidinous, less hot, less loud, less multi-colored, less natural, less robust, and more tasteful, more fluent, more fragile, richer, older, svelter, more elitist, more refined, more disciplined, more cultured, more exquisite, more rigorous, more expensive, more feminine and more delicate (see Fig 2). Similarly, grace compared to sexiness is conceived as less arousing, less extroverted, less libidinous, less hot, less loud, less multi-colored, less robust, and more fluent, more fragile, richer, older, svelter, more elitist, more refined, more disciplined, more cultured, more exquisite, more expensive, more feminine and more delicate.

Notably, all items that distinguish beauty and sexiness likewise distinguish elegance and sexiness; however, the degree of the differences is far greater between elegance and sexiness than between beauty and sexiness. Moreover, there are roughly three times as many semantic differentials showing differences between elegance and sexiness compared to those showing differences for beauty and sexiness. Regarding the semantic differential multi-colored vs. monochrome, ratings for elegance lean far more strongly to the “monochrome” end than those for “beauty” and “sexiness;” at the same time, absolute levels are near the midpoint of the scale, suggesting that elegant objects can well include objects more colorful than the black dress.

Regarding elegance and grace, positive evidence for differences was obtained only for four pairs: grace appears to be conceived as slightly less sober, rigorous, tasteful and expensive than elegance. This corroborates the very close affinity of elegance and grace obtained in the free association task of Study 1. At the same time, the differences we found are well in line with the concept of grace in classical aesthetics. After all, grace––in contrast to elegance––is also attributed to movements of children and hence to phenomena which do not necessarily involve intellectual rigor and “high” taste, let alone high purchasing power. (Accordingly, we had expected grace to differ from elegance on the semantic differential of natural vs. artificial. However, though the descriptive difference was showing the expected pattern of a higher naturalness of grace, Bayes Factors yielded evidence that elegance and grace do not differ in this regard.)

In sum, favoring Lord Kames’ definition of grace as “elegance in motion” over Schiller’s definition as “beauty in motion,” our results show a stronger affinity of grace with elegance than with beauty. At the same time, as both elegance and grace are closely related with beauty, Schiller’s definition is justified as well.

Like in Study 1, we additionally performed separate analyses that tested all four concepts for gender differences. Results show no statistically significant difference; this holds both for each of the 42 semantic differentials when analyzed separately, and for their entirety (for details, see S6 Text). For sexiness, the MANOVA yielded a significant difference between men and women, mostly driven by the items feminine-masculine, with men rating sexiness as being more feminine and women as more masculine, and for tall-short, with women associating sexiness with greater body height (tall) than men.

General discussion

Our study shows that elegance and grace differ very strongly and categorially from sexiness, yet that all these categories still show much overlap with the broader category of beauty. Each of these four categories allows for a precise comparative definition regarding characteristic dimensions of aesthetic appeal and is, moreover, associated with distinct cognitive, affective and partly also social implications.

From a methodological point of view, we would like to note that linguistic corpus analyses alone could barely have tapped into the distinct cognitive and affective profiles of the four concepts under scrutiny in nearly as controlled, directly comparable and multi-dimensional a fashion as their projection onto 42 carefully chosen bipolar dimensions which all allow for subtle graded distinctions on scales ranging from 1 to 7.

Elegance appears to be a particularly fruitful topic for future research, as it is a very distinct aesthetic virtue that is still found across multiple domains of both visual and verbal aesthetics and is lexicalized across many languages and cultures. Reflecting its importance, the concept of elegance is, in fact, frequently used in journals on fashion, design, and architecture. However, previous scientific efforts to explicate the meaning of elegance are largely confined to elegance in scientific proofs and software programming [4854]. In contrast, visual and verbal elegance has to date gone almost completely untreated both in philosophical and in empirical aesthetics. Accordingly, neither the Oxford Encyclopedia of Aesthetics [55] nor a German six-volume dictionary of key concepts of aesthetics [56] include an entry for “elegance.”

The preeminent importance of dimensions of fluency, lightness, delicacy (slenderness), and simplicity for the mental construct of elegance strongly support the notion that the stipulations of the processing fluency or ease of processing hypothesis of aesthetic liking [18] entail great potential for future research specifically on elegance. In a similar vein, grace remains a relevant aesthetic virtue regarding dance and other movements.

In sum, our data strongly suggest that it is time to put hitherto neglected concepts closely related to beauty––specifically, elegance and grace––on the agenda of research in aesthetics. Considering these nuances would help to prevent beauty ratings from being used as too broad and hence too vague a standard currency in empirical aesthetics. Importantly, our study provides concrete guidance as to which rating items could be used to collect multi-item measures of elegance, grace, beauty, and sexiness.

Limitations and future directions

The results obtained in our two studies exclusively reveal the current understanding of the four target terms in a Western cultural context. Specifically, the strong association with intellectual rigor and impressions of simplicity we found in our data for elegance is likely to be influenced by modernist reinterpretations of elegance as in Bauhaus architecture and design and the concomitant distancing from an aesthetics of ornament [57]. Elegant aristocratic fashions were often highly ornamental and playful, and far less purist and austere. In cultures in which modernist design and architecture had no comparable roles, the current understanding of elegance may well be different. On a similar note, the degree to which the concepts of grace and sexiness vary across languages and cultures also warrants further investigation.

Our questionnaire did not specifically target any negative attributions of personality traits that might also be associated with an elegance personal appearance. However, just like physical attractiveness research has revealed not only positive, but also some negative trait attributions dependent on perceived attractiveness ([5861], pp. 241–252), similar adverse effects on person perception may well be obtained for elegance, too, if targeted by appropriate scales.

The null results regarding gender and age differences (as reported in S4 and S6 Texts in the Supporting Information) cannot readily be generalized, as our samples were not balanced concerning these variables. Moreover, in our sample, participants aged 30 and higher make up only 25% of the sample and are hence relatively underrepresented.

Our two studies—though guided by fairly pronounced expectations—are to a large extent exploratory in nature. Testing specific hypotheses derived from our present findings would be a next step. Statistical analyses of elegant objects and features of movement are further very interesting topics for future research.

Considering that elegance is strongly associated with beauty, but that by no means all beauty is elegant, the question arises whether neural correlates of perceiving elegance can partly be dissociated from those that have been reported for perceived beauty [6267].

Last, but not least, our two studies exclusively elucidate the mental constructs which individuals have acquired regarding beauty, elegance, grace, and sexiness. In order to consolidate these findings, the distinct pattern of overlaps and differences between the four categories would need to be replicated by studies in which participants rate preselected sets of beautiful, elegant, graceful and sexy stimuli on a broader range of identical scales.

Conclusion

The present study elucidates affinities and differences between beauty, elegance, grace, and sexiness on a rich set of cognitive, affective, age- and gender-related, and social implications. These comparative distinctions have great potential to help overcome the relative vagueness with which a barely defined concept of beauty is largely dominating empirical studies in aesthetics.

Supporting information

S1 Text. Collocates of the term “elegant”.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0218728.s001

(DOCX)

S2 Text. Details of the participant sample of Study 1.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0218728.s002

(DOCX)

S3 Text. Exploratory analyses of the free association data.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0218728.s003

(DOCX)

S4 Text. Testing for effects of gender and age.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0218728.s004

(DOCX)

S5 Text. Bayes factor analyses for pair wise compassion of the four categories.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0218728.s005

(DOCX)

S6 Text. Analysis of the semantic differential ratings for beauty, elegance, grace, and sexiness by gender.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0218728.s006

(DOCX)

S1 Fig. Dendrograms of hierarchical cluster analyses.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0218728.s007

(PNG)

S2 Fig. Classical multi-dimensional scaling.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0218728.s008

(PNG)

S3 Fig. Visualizing the co-occurrences as a network.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0218728.s009

(PNG)

S2 Table. Descriptive statistics for person variables.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0218728.s013

(XLSX)

S3 Table. Descriptive statistics for elegant diction.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0218728.s014

(XLSX)

S4 Table. Descriptive statistics for elegant objects.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0218728.s015

(XLSX)

S5 Table. Most frequently mentioned elegant persons (celebrities).

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0218728.s016

(XLSX)

S7 Table. Results of pairwise comparisons (MANOVAs) for the seven factors.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0218728.s018

(XLSX)

Acknowledgments

We wish to thank Barbara Budai, Doga Cigsar, Martina Pfeffer, Anna Roth, Sanja Methner, and Lisa Memmeler for their help in data collection in Study 1 and 2, Anjan Chatterjee and Stefan Koelsch for their comments on an earlier version of the manuscript, and Michael Forster for his comments during the review process.

References

  1. 1. Augustin MD, Wagemans J, Carbon C-C. All is beautiful? Generality vs. specificity of word usage in visual aesthetics. Acta Psychol. 2012;139(1):187‒201. pmid:22123506
  2. 2. Knoop CA, Wagner V, Jacobsen T, Menninghaus W. Mapping the aesthetic space of literature “from below”. Poetics. 2016;56:35‒49.
  3. 3. Jacobsen T, Buchta K, Köhler M, Schröger E. The primacy of beauty in judging the aesthetics of objects. Psychol Rep. 2004;94(3):1253‒60. pmid:15362400
  4. 4. Burke E. A philosophical enquiry into the origin of our ideas of the sublime and beautiful. Boulton JT, editor. Notre Dame, IN: University of Notre Dame Press; 1756/1968.
  5. 5. Kant I. Kant: Observations on the feeling of the beautiful and sublime and other writings. Cambridge, England: Cambridge University Press; 1764/2011.
  6. 6. Kant I. Critique of the power of judgment Guyer P, editor. Cambridge, England: Cambridge University Press; 1790/2001.
  7. 7. Ishizu T, Zeki S. A neurobiological enquiry into the origins of our experience of the sublime and beautiful. Front Hum Neurosci. 2014;8. pmid:25426046
  8. 8. Eskine KJ, Kacinik NA, Prinz JJ. Stirring images: Fear, not happiness or arousal, makes art more sublime. Emotion. 2012;12(5):1071‒4. pmid:22309722
  9. 9. Gordon AM, Stellar JE, Anderson CL, McNeil GD, Loew D, Keltner D. The dark side of the sublime: Distinguishing a threat-based variant of awe. J Pers Soc Psychol. 2017;113(2): 310‒28. pmid:27929301
  10. 10. Diderot D. Diderot on Art, Volume II. The Salon of 1767. New Haven: Yale University Press; 1767/1995.
  11. 11. Schlegel F. On the study of Greek poetry. Albany, NY: SUNY Press; 1797/2001.
  12. 12. Fayn K, MacCann C, Tiliopoulos N, Silvia PJ. Aesthetic emotions and aesthetic people: Openness predicts sensitivity to novelty in the experiences of interest and pleasure. Front Psychol. 2015;6. pmid:26696940
  13. 13. Ngai S. Our aesthetic categories: Zany, cute, interesting. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press; 2012.
  14. 14. Silvia PJ. Confusion and interest: The role of knowledge emotions in aesthetic experience. Psychol Aesthet Creat Arts. 2010;4(2):75‒80.
  15. 15. Schindler I, Hosoya G, Menninghaus W, Beermann U, Wagner V, Eid M, et al. Measuring aesthetic emotions: A review of the literature and a new assessment tool. PLoS ONE. 2017;12(6):e0178899. pmid:28582467
  16. 16. Winkielman P, Cacioppo JT. Mind at ease puts a smile on the face: Psychophysiological evidence that processing facilitation elicits positive affect. J Pers Soc Psychol. 2001;81(6):989‒1000. pmid:11761320
  17. 17. Winkielman P, Schwarz N, Fazendeiro T, Reber R. The hedonic marking of processing fluency: Implications for evaluative judgment. In: Musch J, Klauer KC, editors. The psychology of evaluation: Affective processes in cognition and emotion. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum; 2003. p. 189‒217.
  18. 18. Reber R, Schwarz N, Winkielman P. Processing fluency and aesthetic pleasure: Is beauty in the perceiver's processing experience? Pers Soc Psychol Rev. 2004;8(4):364‒82. pmid:15582859
  19. 19. Winkielman P, Halberstadt J, Fazendeiro T, Catty S. Prototypes are attractive because they are easy on the mind. Psychol Sci. 2006;17(9):799‒806. pmid:16984298
  20. 20. Unkelbach C. The Learned Interpretation of Cognitive Fluency. Psychol Sci. 2006;17(4):339–45. pmid:16623692
  21. 21. Silvia PJ. Knowledge-based assessment of expertise in the arts: Exploring aesthetic fluency. Psychol Aesthet Creat Arts. 2007;1(4):247‒9.
  22. 22. Morwood J. The Pocket Oxford Latin dictionary. Oxford: Oxford University Press; 2000. 368 p.
  23. 23. Darwin C. The descent of man, and selection in relation to sex. Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press; 1871/1981.
  24. 24. Schiller F. On grace and dignity. In: Curran JV, Fricker C, editors. Schiller's "On grace and dignity" in its cultural context. Essays and a new translation. Woodbridge, UK: Boydell and Brewer; 1793/2005. p. 123‒70.
  25. 25. Ästhetische Grundbegriffe: Historisches Wörterbuch in sieben Bänden [Basic concepts of aesthetics: Historical dictionary in seven volumes]. Stuttgart, Germany: J. B. Metzler Verlag; 2010. Amut/Grazie; p. 193‒208.
  26. 26. Lord Kames HH. Elements of criticism. 7th ed. Edinburgh: Printed for J. Bell and W. Creech etc.; 1788.
  27. 27. Osgood CE, Suci G, Tannenbaum P. The measurement of meaning. Urbana: University of Illinois Press; 1957.
  28. 28. Fehr B, Russell JA. Concept of emotion viewed from a prototype perspective. J Exp Psychol Gen. 1984;113(3):464‒86.
  29. 29. Kuehnast M, Wagner V, Wassiliwizky E, Jacobsen T, Menninghaus W. Being moved: Linguistic representation and conceptual structure. Front Psychol. 2014;5:1242. pmid:25404924
  30. 30. Cunningham MR. Measuring the physical in physical attractiveness: Quasi-experiments on the sociobiology of female facial beauty. J Pers Soc Psychol. 1986;50(5):925‒35.
  31. 31. Dion K, Berscheid E, Walster E. What is beautiful is good. J Pers Soc Psychol. 1972;24(3):285‒90. pmid:4655540
  32. 32. Eagly AH, Ashmore RD, Makhijani MG, Longo LC. What is beautiful is good, but…: A meta-analytic review of research on the physical attractiveness stereotype. Psychol Bull. 1991;110(1):109‒28.
  33. 33. Sutrop U. List task and a cognitive salience index. Field Methods. 2001;13(3):263‒76.
  34. 34. Forster M, Leder H, Ansorge U. It felt fluent, and I liked it: Subjective feeling of fluency rather than objective fluency determines liking. Emotion. 2013;13(2):280‒9. pmid:23088777
  35. 35. Jakesch M, Leder H, Forster M. Image ambiguity and fluency. PLoS ONE. 2013;8(9):e74084. pmid:24040172
  36. 36. Menninghaus W, Bohrn IC, Knoop CA, Kotz SA, Schlotz W, Jacobs AM. Rhetorical features facilitate prosodic processing while handicapping ease of semantic comprehension. Cognition. 2015;143:48‒60. pmid:26113449
  37. 37. Nunes JC, Ordanini A, Valsesia F. The power of repetition: Repetitive lyrics in a song increase processing fluency and drive market success. J Consum Psychol. 2015;25(2):187‒99.
  38. 38. Bourdieu P. The forms of capital. In: Richardson JG, editor. Handbook of theory and research for the sociology of education. Westport, CT: Greenwood; 1986. p. 241‒58.
  39. 39. Deutschen Gesellschaft für Psychologie e.V. & Berufsverband Deutscher Psychologinnen und Psychologen e.V. Ethische Richtlinien der DGPs und des BDP. 2004.
  40. 40. Revelle W. psych: Procedures for personality and psychological research. 1.7.8. ed. Northwestern University, Evanston, Illinois, USA 2017.
  41. 41. Dziuban CD, Shirkey EC. When is a correlation matrix appropriate for factor analysis? Some decision rules. Psychol Bull. 1974;81(6):358‒61.
  42. 42. Fabrigar LR, Wegener DT, MacCallum RC, Strahan EJ. Evaluating the use of exploratory factor analysis in psychological research. Psychol Meth. 1999;4(3):272‒99.
  43. 43. Horn JL. A rationale and test for the number of factors in factor analysis. Psychometrika. 1965;30(2):179‒85.
  44. 44. Kass RE, Raftery AE. Bayes factors. J Am Stat Assoc. 1995;90(430):773‒95.
  45. 45. Rouder JN, Speckman PL, Sun D, Morey RD, Iverson G. Bayesian t tests for accepting and rejecting the null hypothesis. Psychon Bull Rev. 2009;16(2):225–37. pmid:19293088
  46. 46. Morey RD, Romeijn J-W, Rouder JN. The philosophy of Bayes factors and the quantification of statistical evidence. J Math Psychol. 2016;72:6‒18.
  47. 47. Morey RD, Rouder JN. BayesFactor: Computation of Bayes Factors for Common Designs. 0.9.12–2 ed 2015.
  48. 48. Foster Gage M. Deus ex machina: From semiology to the elegance of aesthetics. Archit Design. 2007;77(1):82‒5.
  49. 49. Goldblatt D. Lightness and fluidity: Remarks concerning the aesthetics of elegance. Archit Design. 2007;77(1):10‒7.
  50. 50. MacLennan BJ. "Who cares about elegance?" The role of aesthetics in programming language design. SIGPLAN Notices. 1997;32(3):33‒7.
  51. 51. Montano U. Case analysis II: Elegance. In: Montano U, editor. Explaining beauty in mathematics: An aesthetic theory of mathematics. Cham, Switzerland: Springer International; 2014. p. 179‒95.
  52. 52. Rahim A, Jamelle H, editors. Architectural Design. Special Issue: Elegance: John Wiley & Sons; 2007.
  53. 53. Salado A, Nilchiani R. Using Maslow's hierarchy of needs to define elegance in system architecture. Procedia Comput Sci. 2013;16:927‒36.
  54. 54. Walsh D. Occam's razor: A principle of intellectual elegance. Am Philos Q. 1979;16(3):241‒4.
  55. 55. Michael K, editor. Encyclopedia of aesthetics. Oxford: Oxford University Press; 1998.
  56. 56. Barck K, Fontius M, Schlenstedt D, Steinwachs B, Wolfzettel F, editors. Ästhetische Grundbegriffe: Historisches Wörterbuch in sieben Bänden. Stuttgart, Germany: J. B. Metzler Verlag; 2010.
  57. 57. Loos A. Ornament und Verbrechen. Wien, Austria: Metroverlag; 2012.
  58. 58. Dermer M, Thiel DL. When beauty may fail. J Pers Soc Psychol. 1975;31(6):1168‒76.
  59. 59. Diener E, Wolsic B, Fujita F. Physical attractiveness and subjective well-being. J Pers Soc Psychol. 1995;69(1):120‒9.
  60. 60. Hatfield E, Sprecher S. Mirror, mirror: The importance of looks in everyday life. Albany, NY: SUNY Press; 1986. 472 p.
  61. 61. Menninghaus W. Das Versprechen der Schönheit. Frankfurt am Main, Germany: Suhrkamp; 2003. 386 p.
  62. 62. Cela-Conde CJ, Ayala FJ. Brain keys in the appreciation of beauty: A tale of two worlds. Rend Fis Acc Lincei. 2014;25(3):277‒84.
  63. 63. Di Dio C, Macaluso E, Rizzolatti G. The golden beauty: Brain response to Classical and Renaissance sculptures. PLoS ONE. 2007;2(11):e1201. pmid:18030335
  64. 64. Jacobs RHAH, Renken R, Cornelissen FW. Neural correlates of visual aesthetics–Beauty as the coalescence of stimulus and internal state. PLoS ONE. 2012;7(2):e31248. pmid:22384006
  65. 65. Kühn S, Gallinat J. The neural correlates of subjective pleasantness. NeuroImage. 2012;61(1):289‒94. pmid:22406357
  66. 66. Vartanian O, Navarrete G, Chatterjee A, Fich LB, Leder H, Modrono C, et al. Impact of contour on aesthetic judgments and approach-avoidance decisions in architecture. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2013;110(Suppl 2):10446‒53. pmid:23754408
  67. 67. Zeki S, Romaya JP, Benincasa DMT, Atiyah MF. The experience of mathematical beauty and its neural correlates. Front Hum Neurosci. 2014;8:68. pmid:24592230