Skip to main content
Advertisement
Browse Subject Areas
?

Click through the PLOS taxonomy to find articles in your field.

For more information about PLOS Subject Areas, click here.

  • Loading metrics

Long rallies and next rally performances in elite men’s and women’s badminton

  • Miguel A. Gomez ,

    Contributed equally to this work with: Miguel A. Gomez, Anthony S. Leicht, Fernando Rivas, Philip Furley

    Roles Conceptualization, Data curation, Formal analysis, Funding acquisition, Investigation, Methodology, Project administration, Resources, Software, Supervision, Validation, Visualization, Writing – original draft, Writing – review & editing

    magor_2@yahoo.es, miguelangel.gomez.ruano@upm.es

    Affiliation Faculty of Physical Activity and Sports Sciences, Department of Social Sciences, Physical Activity, Sport and Leisure, Polytechnic University of Madrid, Madrid, Spain

  • Anthony S. Leicht ,

    Contributed equally to this work with: Miguel A. Gomez, Anthony S. Leicht, Fernando Rivas, Philip Furley

    Roles Formal analysis, Investigation, Methodology, Supervision, Visualization, Writing – original draft, Writing – review & editing

    Affiliation Sport and Exercise Science, James Cook University, Townsville, Queensland, Australia

  • Fernando Rivas ,

    Contributed equally to this work with: Miguel A. Gomez, Anthony S. Leicht, Fernando Rivas, Philip Furley

    Roles Conceptualization, Data curation, Project administration, Resources, Validation

    Affiliations Faculty of Physical Activity and Sports Sciences, Department of Social Sciences, Physical Activity, Sport and Leisure, Polytechnic University of Madrid, Madrid, Spain, Spanish Badminton Federation, Madrid, Spain

  • Philip Furley

    Contributed equally to this work with: Miguel A. Gomez, Anthony S. Leicht, Fernando Rivas, Philip Furley

    Roles Conceptualization, Supervision, Writing – original draft, Writing – review & editing

    Affiliation Institute of Training and Computer Science in Sport, German Sport University Cologne, Cologne, Germany

Abstract

The aim of the present study was twofold: (i) to identify contextual variables associated with the occurrence of long rallies while investigating time-related and technical parameters; and (ii) to identify performance differences between long rallies and the subsequent rally when accounting for match-context and the players’ sex. The sample included 60 men’s (n = 4,475 rallies) and 60 women’s (n = 4,490 rallies) matches randomly selected from the 2015 World Badminton Super Series and World Championship (the final sample included long rallies that had an immediate next point played: n = 1,734 and n = 1,644 rallies for male and female players, respectively). The long rallies represented 19.4% (n = 867) and 16.5% (n = 822) of total rallies for male and female players, respectively. Long rallies were established using a two-step cluster model based on rally time and number of strokes for male (13-79s, 14–72 strokes) and female players (11-56s, 11–52 strokes). The variables collected were point outcome (when serving and receiving, winner, forced-error and unforced-error), number of strokes per rally, rally time, rest time, density, and time between strokes. The rallies were classified into different contexts (clusters) according to influencing factors with eight clusters for male players and three clusters for female players identified. Comparisons among clusters were conducted using Kruskal Wallis and one-way ANOVAs. Comparisons between long and immediate next points were conducted using the Wilcoxon tests for most variables and Crosstabs Command for point outcome and rallies (long and immediate next). Statistically significant differences were identified for both sexes among clusters only for time-related variables (i.e., rally time, rest time, density and time between strokes). In addition, a greater number of strokes, longer rally, rest time, and higher density were identified during long rallies compared with the immediate next rally for both men’s and women’s matches (p<0.05). The time between strokes during long rallies was significantly greater for male players during clusters 3, 5, 6, and 7 (p<0.05) and significantly lower for female players during all clusters (p<0.05). Significant relationships were identified between winning point outcome, and more unforced errors when serving during the immediate next rally (men’s cluster 5 and women’s cluster 2), and more winners when serving during the immediate next rally (men’s cluster 6). The current study identified and characterised long rallies in elite men´s and women´s badminton matches highlighting the importance of sex and contextual factors on time-related and technical demands. Information obtained from these unique sequences of play (i.e., long and immediate next rallies) will assist coaches when modelling and simulating players’ performances (i.e., physiologically and cognitively) during athlete preparation/competition.

Introduction

Prior research in elite badminton has primarily focused on physiological, anthropometrical, biomechanical, visual fitness, anticipatory skill, and temporal and notational analyses [1]. In particular, performance analysis has been widely used to understand the time-related factors of the game (e.g., rally duration, resting times, frequencies between strokes or density of play) that allow for designing training scenarios and drills that simulate the competitive requirements and the high intensity actions of badminton rallies [13]. Chiminazzo, Barreira, Luz, Saraiva, and Cayres [4] reported that a badminton match consisted of high-intensity and short-duration intermittent efforts, interspersed by brief periods of rest. During rallies, numerous motor actions including racquet strokes occur as a result of player displacements and tactical actions (Seth, 2016). These technical, tactical, and timing variables have been the focus of investigation during badminton matches [1], showing, perhaps not surprisingly, that longer rallies coincided with greater rest time, and ultimately longer matches [5]. The inclusion of longer rallies with a greater number of quicker strokes per rally result in a more challenging physical and technical demanding performance during matches [5]. Further, Abian-Vicen, Castanedo, Abian, and Sampedro [6] argued that the highly demanding structure (increased frequency of strokes per second) and heightened work density of rallies reduced the decision time between points and placed greater cognitive load on players during badminton matches.

More recently, Laffaye et al. [3] identified that the temporal structure of elite badminton was becoming more intense (i.e. 34% increase in stroke frequency, 0.9/s– 1.3/s and 5.5–10.2 strokes per rally; and a 34.5% increase in playing time). In fact, the rhythm of strokes and the rally duration has increased substantially during matches with an increase number of long rallies (>19s) accounting for 15% of the total number of rallies played during matches. Consequently, badminton players have to deal with different competitive scenarios (i.e., long and regular duration rallies) that impose challenging physiological and psychological demands that may affect consecutive performances during matches/games. To date, the effect of long rallies and their immediate effect on subsequent rallies/performance is largely unknown. To our knowledge, only one study has investigated this issue via observational analysis of a small sample of 17 matches during the 1996 European Badminton Circuit Tournament at Lisburn, Ireland 1996 [7]. Based upon these matches, rallies undertaken immediately after long rallies were shorter (i.e., 2-3s shorter mean rally length) mainly as a result of unforced errors [7]. The authors highlighted the importance of understanding the time-related factors of badminton matches when preparing players as they may be affected by physical and cognitive fatigue, particularly after long rallies [7]. In fact, Abian et al. [6] argued that coaches need to be aware of the different point scenarios (i.e., rally duration and timing demands) that occur during the match with sound research based on adequate sample sizes needed to assist coaches in simulating training scenarios that have the potential to mitigate debilitating effects of strenuous long rallies on subsequent rallies. During badminton matches, players are confronted with a combination of rapid decision-making, motor performances, pressure (e.g., higher under critical scenarios), and physiological arousal (e.g., higher with intermittent efforts) [8]. For example, pressure can lead to cognitive anxiety that potentially increases during highly demanding contexts such as long rallies during badminton matches given the high physical and physiological demands [9]. These greater demands may also have an impact after a long rally and contribute to debilitating performances as the match progresses. Therefore, understanding how the physical (i.e., time-related parameters of player’s workload such as rally time, rest time, density of play, and time between strokes) and cognitive demands (i.e., the correct technical-tactical decision and execution to win the rally) of competition influence elite badminton players’ performances is crucial in developing better preparatory regimes for players to succeed during competition.

Prior studies examining elite badminton game characteristics have focused on isolated match analysis [1,3,4] and did not account for time-related and technical variables, or the specific contextual variables (i.e. set number, interval of points, score-line or match duration) that result from high-intensity actions (e.g. long rallies and the immediate next rally). Specifically, little attention was given to performance variability during matches and its influence on point outcome [4]. Therefore, the present study attempted to account for these shortcomings of prior research by exploring the time-related and technical aspects of long rallies and their immediate impact on the next rally while accounting for competition context (i.e. contextual variables). More specifically, the present research sought (i) to identify the contexts in which long rallies occur while differentiating time-related and technical parameters; and (ii) to identify performance differences between long rallies and the immediate next rally played. We hypothesized that subsequent rallies played immediately after a long rally would involve lower time-related parameters (e.g., rally time, rest time, frequency, density and frequency between strokes) and more unforced errors modulated by competition contextual factors.

Material and methods

Sample

The sample comprised of 60 matches each for male and female players (n = 120; n = 4,475 and n = 4,490 rallies, respectively) randomly selected from the total number of matches played during the 2015 World Badminton Super Series and World Championship season (n = 449 and n = 434, for men´s and women´s respectively) that met the following criteria: (i) publicly available on TV; (ii) included all rallies played within the full match without omissions; and (iii) included both regular and playoff/knockout stage matches. To account for the disparity of match-performances in elite badminton [10], analyses accounted for match time-duration and time-related structure of rallies for each sex. Therefore, a two-step cluster analysis was conducted (split by sex) initially using number of strokes and rally time as quantitative variables to determine the final sample in accordance with the specificities of long rallies [3]. The models obtained were very good (Silhouette measures > 0.8) and identified two types of rallies played: long (24.5% of the total rallies with rally duration between 13 and 79s, and a mean of 21.1±8.2s; number of strokes between 14- and 72 strokes and a mean of 22.3±8.0 strokes for male players; and 19.6% of the total rallies played with rally duration between 11 and 56s, and a mean of 19.7±6.4s; and number of strokes between 11 and 52, and a mean of 19.4±6.1 for female players); and regular (75.5% of the total rallies played with rally duration between 0 and 12s and a mean of 6.3±3.1s; and number of strokes between 1 and 13, and a mean of 6.8±3.4 strokes for men’s players; and 80.4% of the total rallies with rally duration between 1 and 11s, and a mean of 6.5±3.1s; and number of strokes between 1 and 10, and a mean of 6.1±3.2 for women’s players) rallies. The final sample included only the long rallies that had an immediate next point played (i.e. repeated measure): n = 1,734 and n = 1,644 rallies for male and female players, respectively. The long rallies represented 19.4% (n = 867) and 16.5% (n = 822) of total rallies played for male and female players, respectively. All data were obtained from publicly available sites in accordance with the European Data Protection Law and the current study was conducted with the approval of the Polytechnic University of Madrid Ethics Committee.

Procedure

Several initial stages were conducted to validate data and define/identify variables. Firstly, Stage 1 involved the collation of the sample where match analysis was carried out using a video analysis program (Dartfish, Friburgo, Switzerland) by four observers (Sports Scientists with ten years of experience as elite badminton coaches) trained to collect match temporal and notational variables. The inter- and intra-observer reliability for match analysis was assessed as good to very good (Kappa = >0.81; Pearson’s correlation coefficient: r>0.86; Intra-Class-Correlation coefficient, ICC = >0.85, and the typical error of measurement, TEM = <0.46) [1112] from a selection (n = 12) of randomly selected matches.

Stage 2 included the identification of variables for analysis from matches. The main independent variable studied was long rally and the immediate next rally while the dependent variables (see definitions in Table 1) were related to the time-related factors of the game: rally time, rest time, density, and time between strokes; and the technical variables: number of strokes per rally and rally outcome.

thumbnail
Table 1. Time-related factors, technical and contextual variables analysed.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0229604.t001

In addition, as contextual variables have a direct impact on player´s performances in racket sports [13], the current analyses controlled for total match duration, the influence of match set (1st, 2nd or 3rd), set interval (before and after the regular time-out within each set), and score-line (point differences). These contextual variables from all matches were established as part of Stage 3 with clustering procedures run (two-step cluster analysis) to classify and redefine the variables of match type and score-line [13]. Further, the total match duration (minutes) was examined and classified matches into long and regular ones (see Table 1). The difference in the score between players was established as the score-line, which considered the influence of score (i.e., pressure) on the subsequent performances (long and immediate rally). Finally, the clustering analysis (see Table 1) identified 3 score-lines for both sexes: losing, balanced, or winning by 2 (male players) or 3 points (female players).

Statistical analysis

Firstly, due to the use of match-contextual variables, the sample was split into different clusters that exemplified the specificities of competition (i.e. location and length of long rallies). Then, two-step cluster analyses were conducted separately for each sex including the following variables: set, interval, score-line and match type. This statistical model determined the best number of rally groups (clusters) using the Schwartz’s Bayesian Information Criterion (BIC = 2388.03 and 4377.71, for men´s and women´s rallies, respectively). Additionally, the log-likelihood distance measure was used to check for similarities between rally groups. The models obtained were good with Silhouette measures of 0.85 and 0.50, respectively, for men’s and women’s’ rallies. Accordingly, the sample was split into eight groups for men’s long rallies and three groups for women’s long rallies.

Secondly, the data normality for continuous variables was assessed using the Shapiro-Wilk and Kolmogorov-Smirnov’s tests (p<0.05). Only time between strokes during long rallies (both in men´s and women´s matches) displayed a normal distribution (p>0.05) and one-way ANOVA (Bonferroni´s post hoc test) examined time between strokes among clusters. Non-parametric tests compared performance differences between long and next rallies (repeated measures Wilcoxon test for rally time, rest time, density, number of strokes, and time between strokes) and performance differences among clusters (Kruskal Wallis test). The r effect size (ES) magnitude was calculated and interpreted as follows: small (0–0.30), moderate (0.31–0.50), or large (>0.50) [14].

Thirdly, the relationships between clusters obtained and rally outcome, and long and next rally variables and rally outcomes were determined via Pearson’s Chi-square tests (Crosstabs Command) with adjusted residuals (AR) > 1.96 considered as significant. In addition, Cramer’s V test was used to calculate ES with interpretations based on the following values: 0.10 = small effect, 0.30 = medium effect, and 0.50 = large effect [14]. All analyses were conducted using IBM SPSS for Windows version 23 (IBM. Corp. Armonk, NY) and significance level was set to 0.05.

Results

Table 2 shows the results (distribution of rallies) of the two-step cluster analyses for men’s and women’s rallies. The clustering model identified eight different contexts (clusters) for long rallies during men’s matches. The most frequent context was cluster 5 (18.5%, during 1st set of regular matches, after the interval and when losing), cluster 2 (15.7%, during 2nd set of regular matches, after the interval and during balanced score-lines), cluster 1 (15.3%, during 1st set of regular matches, before the interval and during balanced score-lines), cluster 3 (13.3%, during 3rd set of long matches, after the interval and during balanced score-lines), cluster 8 (11.9%, during 1st set of regular matches, after the interval and during balanced score-lines), and cluster 4 (10.8%, 1st set of regular matches, before the interval and when winning). In addition, clusters 6 (7.2%, during 1st set of long matches, before the interval and during balanced score-lines) and 7 (7.4%, during 3rd set of long matches, before the interval and during balanced score-lines) were less frequent with these occurring specifically during long matches. During women’s matches, only three contexts (clusters) where long rallies happened were identified (Table 3). Cluster 3 was the most prevalent (38.7% during the 1st set of long matches, before the interval and during balanced score-lines) while cluster 1 (33.3% during 3rd set of regular matches, after the interval and during balanced score-lines) and 2 (28.0% during 1st set of long matches, after the interval and when losing) were also characterised.

thumbnail
Table 2. Results of the long rally groups (clusters) identified by the two-step cluster analysis for men’s and women’s players (I = importance of variables when classifying long rallies; BIC = Schwartz’s Bayesian Information Criterion).

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0229604.t002

thumbnail
Table 3. Results of Kruskal Wallis (χ2) and one-way ANOVA (F-value) tests differentiating time-related factors among clusters for men´s and women´s long rallies.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0229604.t003

Significant differences among clusters for rally time, rest time, density and time between strokes were identified for men’s and women’s long rallies (p<0.05; see Table 3).

The pairwise comparisons showed clear differences among clusters in men´s long rallies (see Table 5) with cluster 6 as the longest rally and clusters 1 and 8 as the shortest ones; clusters 3 and 7 were the ones with longer rest time and cluster 8 involved less rest time; clusters 4 and 8 were of higher density and clusters 3 and 7 were of lower density; and clusters 1 and 2 involved less time between strokes. For women´s long rallies, cluster 1 showed greater time between strokes, rally time and rest time, and higher density than clusters 2 and 3 (see Table 5).

No significant (p>0.05) relationships were identified among clusters for point outcome for both men’s and women´s matches (Table 4).

thumbnail
Table 4. Frequency distribution of point outcome for each cluster during men’s and women´s matches.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0229604.t004

During long rallies of men’s matches, rally time, rest time, density and time between strokes were significantly (all p<0.05) greater compared to the next rally for all clusters (Table 5). Similarly, the frequency of strokes was significantly greater during long rallies compared to the next rally but only for clusters 3, 5, 6 and 7 (all p<0.01, Table 5). Large effects (ES) were noted for rally time and rest time variables in clusters 2, 3, 5 and 8, and for density in clusters 2 and 8 (Table 5).

thumbnail
Table 5. Descriptive statistics (median and interquartile range) for time-related factors during long and next rallies during men’s matches.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0229604.t005

During long rallies of women’s matches, rally time, rest time, density, and time between strokes were significantly (all p<0.05) greater compared to the next rally for all clusters (Table 6). However, the time between strokes was significantly lower during long rallies compared to the next rally for all clusters (all p<0.05, Table 6). Large effects (ES) were noted for rally time, rest time, and density variables in all clusters (Table 6).

thumbnail
Table 6. Descriptive statistics (median and interquartile range) for time-related factors during long and next rallies during women’s matches.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0229604.t006

Significant relationships between rallies and point outcome for men’s matches were identified (Table 7) for two specific match contexts: (i) during cluster 5 (small ES = 0.22) with more points won by the serving player during long rallies compared to the next rallies (26.3% vs. 15.0%; AR = 2.5), and with more points won via unforced errors when serving during the next rallies compared to the long rallies (23.1% vs. 10.0%; AR = 3.2); and (ii) during cluster 6 (moderate ES = 0.35) with more points won by the receiving player during the next rallies compared to the long rallies (25.8% vs. 8.1%; AR = 2.6), and with more points won via unforced errors when receiving during long rallies compared to next rallies (33.9% vs. 17.7%; AR = 2.1).

thumbnail
Table 7. Frequency distribution of point outcome for long rally and the next rally according to each cluster during men’s matches.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0229604.t007

Significant relationships between rallies and point outcome for women’s matches were identified (Table 8) during cluster 2 (small ES = 0.18) with more points won for the serving player during long rallies compared to the next rallies (25.2% vs. 17.0%; AR = 2.2), and with more points won via unforced errors when receiving during the next rallies compared to long rallies (22.2% vs. 12.6%; AR = 2.7).

thumbnail
Table 8. Frequency distribution of point outcome for long rally and the next rally according to each cluster in women’s matches.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0229604.t008

Discussion

The aim of the current study was two-fold: (i) to identify the contexts where long rallies occur while differentiating time-related and technical parameters; and (ii) to identify performance differences between long rallies and the subsequent rally played according to the match-context and players’ sex. The current findings highlighted the importance of separating analyses by sex and the need to consider the specificities of match-contexts. In particular, men and women’s matches exhibited different long rally contexts with eight and three clusters, respectively [10]. Specifically, clear differences among clusters were identified for time-related parameters (i.e., rally and rest time, density and time between strokes) indicating the specific and independent scenarios where long rallies occur based on contextual variables. In addition, different performances were noted during long and the immediate next rallies from time-related (i.e., rally time, rest time, strokes per rally, density, and time between strokes) and technical (i.e., different point outcome after long rally for men’s players in clusters 5 and 6, and for women’s players in cluster 2) points of view. Therefore, our results confirm our hypothesis that rallies played immediately after a long rally involve lower time-related parameters and more unforced errors modulated by several contextual factors.

One intriguing finding of the current study was the distribution of total long rallies in men and women’s matches. The frequency of these high-demanding rallies was 24.5% during men’s matches and 19.6% during women’s matches, values greater than the 15% reported by Laffaye et al. [3] during the London 2012 Olympic Games. Our results confirm the current trends of the intermittent and high intensity demands required by players during elite badminton matches and the increasing number of long rallies played during matches by both sexes [10]. Further, the current findings corroborate that elite badminton matches mainly consist of short-intermittent rallies (around 75% and 80% of men’s and women’s matches, respectively) interspersed with long rallies that modify players’ demands [35]. Further, this finding points out the necessity in performance analyses to scrutinize published reports with contemporary data as sports like badminton constantly evolve with different demands exhibited by players that need to be taken into account during training and coaching practices.

Furthermore, the contexts (clusters) in which the long rallies occurred differed according to sex and match-contextual variables, reflecting the importance of match scenarios that modify players’ responses and their subsequent performances [10,13]. In fact, male players experienced a greater variability of contexts where long rallies occurred (clusters 1, 2, 4, 5 and 8 during 1st and 2nd sets of regular matches; and clusters 3, 6 and 7 during 1st and 3rd sets of long matches) compared to female players who experienced only three main contexts, two during long matches (clusters 2 and 3 during 1st set) and one during regular matches (cluster 1 during 3rd set). These findings reflected that clusters were different for time-related parameters (i.e., rally time, rest time, density and time between strokes) with each context distinctively affecting player´s performance. Abdullahi et al. [2] recommended that coaches and players should focus on these time-related performance indicators as evidence-based guidelines for training and match preparation. Therefore, our results provide further support that consideration of these match-context constraints is extremely important to understand the differences between long rallies and immediate next rallies from a time-related and technical analysis approach [34,15]. Consideration of these contextual influences would allow modelling of match perturbations or critical incidents to identify potential player performance decrements [16] and training strategies to enhance players’ focus during different match stages (intervals, sets) that are comparable to real scenarios [17].

For men’s matches, significantly greater rally time, rest time, density, and strokes per rally were identified during long rallies compared to the next rally for all clusters. Additionally, the time between strokes was higher during long rallies (i.e. increased intensity with shorter time between strokes) compared to the next rally for clusters 3, 5, 6, and 7. These findings may support Liddle and O’Donoghue’s [7] conclusions that rallies following a long rally were shorter and less intense as a result of physical and mental fatigue after the prior long effort. Subsequently, players may feel more fatigued due to the high-intensity effort during a long rally and undertake more rest time to recover for the next rally. This may also be a critical time of the match and therefore players need to be well prepared for these kinds of situations [7].

For women’s matches, a divergent result was observed with long rallies consisting of less time between strokes for all match contexts (clusters 1, 2, and 3) despite greater rally time, rest time, density, and strokes per rally. This result may indicate that female players increase the intensity of play immediately after a long rally, trying to take advantage of the potential fatigue produced during the long rally. Further, it may also indicate that female players pace themselves during long rallies to minimise physical fatigue [7] and thereby might have more reserves to maintain the technical, tactical, physical, and psychological aspects for matches [16]. In particular, the combination of accumulative physical and mental fatigue may produce different tactics from players during each competition scenario including long and immediate next rallies to minimise debilitating performances (e.g., higher time between shots after long rallies) as the set/match goes on [9]. On the other hand, the relationship between clusters and point outcome was not significant. This may reflect that players perform with different physical intensities (i.e., time-related factors) during long rallies that does not directly affect point outcome. However, some differences were identified when accounting for context variables in assessing the relationship between long rally and the immediate next rally. In particular, only two specific contexts during men’s matches (clusters 5 and 6) and only one context during women’s matches (cluster 2) were significant. These findings highlight the importance of controlling for contextual variables as these have the potential to modify time-related factors of long rallies during men´s (balanced and losing score-lines during both intervals in regular and long matches) and women´s (interval of 11 to 21 points when losing and playing long matches) matches [10,13]. Further, this indirectly demonstrates the importance of simulating specific match scenarios in practice or creating certain scenarios within a match (e.g., forcing opponent’s errors after a stressful/long rally). Liddle and O’Donoghue [7] reported that more unforced errors occurred due to physical and mental fatigue during next rallies after long rallies. Results for men’s cluster 5 and women’s cluster 2, where players won more points when serving during the long rally but lost the immediate next rally via an unforced error, provided further support of this fatigue-induced reasoning. More specifically, this pattern of results might have been induced by physical fatigue during high-competitive conditions [89] which in turn impacted on players’ cognitive performance during and after long rallies (e.g., visual search, anticipation or decision-making selecting the correct technique/ response). However, the opposite pattern occurred within men’s cluster 6 (with the longest rally time of all clusters) where players won long rallies via the opponent’s unforced errors and then won the immediate next rally via a clear winning shot. In this context, male players may have taken advantage of their opponent’s early 1st set mistakes and fatigue-onset during high-demanding, long rallies (e.g. balanced score-lines of long matches) by attacking the immediate next point due to their own reduced physical/cognitive fatigue (i.e. better physical/cognitive fitness). These results clearly highlight the importance of analysing the physical and psychological demands that players experience during different scenarios (i.e., clusters) of the set and/or match [9].

The current results are helpful in developing specific practical applications for coaches and athletes in both training interventions and match tactics and strategies [18]. Specifically, for women’s matches, we would recommend that players undertake simulated intermittent activity that replicates long and immediate next rallies during the 3rd set of regular matches (dealing with winning and close score-lines) and 1st set of long matches (both losing and winning score-lines). Such training simulations would better prepare players physiologically and cognitively for these competitive scenarios during matches. For men’s matches, we recommend that players consider the importance of higher frequencies of strokes during long rallies with match contexts similar to clusters 3, 5, 6, and 7. Specifically, training drills should control for fatigue effects in the immediate next rally during simulations of the 1st set of regular matches (losing during the interval of points 11 to 21) and long matches (with balanced score-lines and interval of points from 0 to 11). The use of a minimum number of strokes and range of rally durations (under fatigue conditions) would help better prepare players so that they perform with less unforced errors during and after long rallies.

To our knowledge, the current study was the first to explore time-related and technical demands of long rallies and their immediate impact on the next rally in elite badminton while considering contextual factors. However, some limitations of the present research need to be considered for further studies. Firstly, the variables examined in this study were limited to those reported in previous studies, while future studies might include further technical (e.g., technical actions used during both rallies), tactical (e.g., zones of the court used to start and end both rallies), time-related (e.g., accumulated playing time when the long rally occurs), and match-contextual (i.e., quality of opposition) variables. Secondly, the examination of matches during consecutive tournaments (e.g. World Championships or Olympic Games) may produce different performance trends during long and immediate next rallies, and, hence, should be considered in forthcoming research. Thirdly, matches were randomly selected from major tournaments and included both regular/group and knockout/elimination matches. As only one study investigated the differences between stages [4] this study was a preliminary investigation with future studies of distinct stages recommended. Additionally, future studies might want to adopt a multivariate approach to define the match-context and performance influences during long rallies in elite badminton to assist coaches with player preparation [1819].

In conclusion, the current study identified and characterised long rallies in elite men´s and women´s badminton matches highlighting the importance of sex and contextual factors on time-related and technical demands. The results identified eight and three different contexts of long rallies for men and women’s matches, respectively. Specifically, men´s clusters 1 and 2 (longer time between strokes), 6 (longer rally time), 7 (longer rest time) and 8 (shorter rally time); and women´s cluster 1 (longer rally time and rest time, and longer time between strokes) demonstrated clear and different time-related demands than the other clusters for their sex. Further, different time-related factors and point outcome of long rallies and the immediate next rally were identified for both sexes. In particular, different time between strokes were identified during next rallies compared to long rallies that varied with match context (e.g. longer in women’s matches after the long rally and shorter in men’s matches after long rallies). Additionally, the point outcome was associated with an increased number of unforced errors after playing long rallies in men’s cluster 5 and women’s cluster 2. This research knowledge is essential and innovative in order to have a better understanding of the time-related and technical demands of elite badminton where intermittent and different critical scenarios occur [4,15]. The information obtained from these sequences of play (i.e., long and immediate next rallies) will assist coaches in their modelling and simulation of players’ performances (i.e., physiologically and cognitively) during these unique match contexts [3,18].

Supporting information

S1 Dataset. IBM SPSS dataset of men´s badminton players.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0229604.s001

(SAV)

S2 Dataset. IBM SPSS dataset of women´s badminton players.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0229604.s002

(SAV)

Acknowledgments

This study was supported by a mobility Grant “Salvador de Madariaga” (PRX18/00098; Ministry of Education, Culture and Sport of Spain) for the corresponding author during a research exchange at James Cook University (Townsville, Australia).

References

  1. 1. Phomsoupha M, Laffaye G. The science of badminton: game characteristics, anthropometry, physiology, visual fitness and biomechanics. Sport Med. 2015; 45(4): 473–495.
  2. 2. Abdullahi Y, Coetzee B. Notational singles match analysis of male badminton players who participated in the African Badminton Championships. Int J Perf Anal Sport. 2017; 17(1–2): 1–16.
  3. 3. Laffaye G, Phomsoupha M, Dor F. Changes in the game characteristics of a badminton match: a longitudinal study through the olympic game finals analysis in men’s singles. J Sport Sci Med. 2015; 14(3): 584–590.
  4. 4. Chiminazzo JGC, Barreira J, Luz LS, Saraiva WC, Cayres JT. Technical and timing characteristics of badminton men’s single: comparison between groups and play-offs stages in 2016 Rio Olympic Games. Int J Perf Anal Sport. 2018; 18(2): 245–254.
  5. 5. Abián P, Castanedo A, Feng XQ, Sampedro J, Abian-Vicen J. Notational comparison of men’s singles badminton matches between Olympic Games in Beijing and London. Int J Perf Anal Sport. 2014; 14(1): 42–53.
  6. 6. Abian-Vicen J, Castanedo A, Abian P, Sampedro J. Temporal and notational comparison of badminton matches between men’s singles and women’s singles. Int J Perf Anal Sport. 2013; 13(2): 310–320.
  7. 7. Liddle D, O’Donoghue P. Notational Analysis of rallies in European circuit badminton. In Lees A., Maynard I., Hughes M., Reilly T. (eds.), Science and Racket Sports II (pp. 275–281). E and FN Spon, London: 1998.
  8. 8. Alder DB, Broadbent DP, Stead J, Poolton J. The impact of physiological load on anticipation skills in badminton: From testing to training. J Sport Sci. 2019; Ahead of print: pmid:30931825
  9. 9. Duncan MJ, Chan CK, Clarke ND, Cox M, Smith M. The effect of badminton-specific exercise on badminton short-serve performance in competition and practice climates. Eur J Sport Sci. 2017; 17(2): 119–126. pmid:27412037
  10. 10. Torres-Luque G, Fernández-García ÁI, Blanca-Torres JC, Kondric M, Cabello-Manrique D. Statistical differences in set analysis in badminton at the RIO 2016 Olympic Games. Frontier Psych. 2019; 10: 731. pmid:31001177
  11. 11. Altman DG. Mathematics for kappa. Practical Statistics for Medical Research (pp. 406–407). Chapman & Hall London, UK: 1991.
  12. 12. Hopkins WG. Measures of reliability in sports medicine and science. Sport Med. 2000; 30: 1–15.
  13. 13. Gómez MÁ, García-de-Alcaráz A, Furley P. Analysis of contextual-related variables on serve and receiving performances in elite men’s and women’s table tennis players. Int J Perf Anal Sport. 2017; 17(6): 919–933.
  14. 14. Field A. Discovering Statistics using IBM SPSS Statistics (4th ed). Sage, London: 2013.
  15. 15. Barreira J, Chiminazzo JGC, Fernandes PT. Analysis of point difference established by winners and losers in games of badminton. Int J Perf Anal Sport. 2016; 16(2): 687–694.
  16. 16. Hughes M, Hughes MT, Behan H. The evolution of computerised notational analysis through the example of racket sports. Int J Sport Sci Engin. 2007; 1(1): 3–28.
  17. 17. Perl J. Artificial neural networks in sports: New concepts and approaches. Int J Perf Anal Sport. 2001; 1(1): 106–121.
  18. 18. Gawin W, Beyer C, Seidler M. A competition analysis of the single and double disciplines in world-class badminton. Int J Perf Anal Sport. 2015; 15(3): 997–1006.
  19. 19. Seth B. Determination factors of badminton game performance. Int J Phys Ed Sport Health. 2016; 3: 20–22.