Skip to main content
Advertisement
Browse Subject Areas
?

Click through the PLOS taxonomy to find articles in your field.

For more information about PLOS Subject Areas, click here.

  • Loading metrics

Acceptance and knowledge of evolutionary theory among third-year university students in Spain

  • Juan Gefaell,

    Roles Conceptualization, Data curation, Formal analysis, Investigation, Methodology, Writing – review & editing

    Affiliation Facultade de Bioloxía, Universidade de Vigo, Campus de Vigo, Vigo, Spain

  • Tamara Prieto,

    Roles Methodology, Resources, Software

    Affiliation Centro de Investigacións Biomédicas (CINBIO), Universidade de Vigo, Campus de Vigo, Vigo, Spain

  • Mohamed Abdelaziz,

    Roles Investigation, Writing – review & editing

    Affiliation Departamento de Genética, Universidad de Granada, Avda Fuentenueva, Granada, Spain

  • Inés Álvarez,

    Roles Investigation, Writing – review & editing

    Affiliation Real Jardín Botánico, Consejo Superior de Investigaciones Científicas (CSIC), Pza. de Murillo, Madrid, Spain

  • Josefa Antón,

    Roles Investigation, Writing – review & editing

    Affiliation Departamento de Fisiología, Genética y Microbiología, Universidad de Alicante, Apdo., Alicante, Spain

  • Juan Arroyo,

    Roles Investigation, Writing – review & editing

    Affiliation Departamento de Biología Vegetal y Ecología, Universidad de Sevilla, Sevilla, Spain

  • Jose L. Bella,

    Roles Investigation, Validation, Writing – review & editing

    Affiliation Departamento de Biología (Genética), Facultad de Ciencias, Universidad Autónoma de Madrid, Madrid, Spain

  • Miguel Botella,

    Roles Validation, Writing – review & editing

    Affiliation Departamento de Medicina Legal, Toxicología y Antropología Física, Universidad de Granada, Parque Tecnológico de la Salud, Av. de la Investigación, Granada, Spain

  • Anxela Bugallo,

    Roles Investigation, Writing – review & editing

    Affiliation Departamento de Pedagogía y Didáctica, Universidade da Coruña, Campus de Elviña, A Coruña, Spain

  • Vicente Claramonte,

    Roles Investigation, Writing – review & editing

    Affiliation Unidad Docente de Lógica y Filosofía de la Ciencia, Departamento de Filosofía, Facultad de Filosofía y Ciencias de la Educación, Universitat de València, Valencia, Spain

  • José Gijón,

    Roles Validation, Writing – review & editing

    Affiliation Departamento de Didáctica y Organización Escolar, Universidad de Granada, Campus de Cartuja, Granada, Spain

  • Emilio Lizarte,

    Roles Investigation, Writing – review & editing

    Affiliation Departamento de Didáctica y Organización Escolar, Universidad de Granada, Campus de Cartuja, Granada, Spain

  • Rosa M. Maroto,

    Roles Validation, Writing – review & editing

    Affiliation Departamento de Medicina Legal, Toxicología y Antropología Física, Universidad de Granada, Parque Tecnológico de la Salud, Av. de la Investigación, Granada, Spain

  • Manuel Megías,

    Roles Conceptualization, Validation, Writing – review & editing

    Affiliation Facultade de Bioloxía, Universidade de Vigo, Campus de Vigo, Vigo, Spain

  • Borja Milá,

    Roles Investigation, Writing – review & editing

    Affiliation Museo Nacional de Ciencias Naturales, Consejo Superior de Investigaciones Científicas (CSIC), Madrid, Spain

  • Cori Ramón,

    Roles Investigation, Writing – review & editing

    Affiliation Departament de Biología, Universitat de les Illes Balears, Palma de Mallorca, Carretera de Valldemossa, Illes Balears, Spain

  • Marta Vila,

    Roles Investigation, Writing – review & editing

    Affiliation Departamento de Bioloxía, Facultade de Ciencias, Universidade da Coruña, Campus da Zapateira, A Coruña, Spain

  •  [ ... ],
  • Emilio Rolán-Alvarez

    Roles Formal analysis, Funding acquisition, Project administration, Supervision, Visualization, Writing – original draft, Writing – review & editing

    rolan@uvigo.es

    Affiliation Facultade de Bioloxía, Universidade de Vigo, Campus de Vigo, Vigo, Spain

  • [ view all ]
  • [ view less ]

Abstract

The theory of evolution is one of the greatest scientific achievements in the intellectual history of humankind, yet it is still contentious within certain social groups. Despite being as robust and evidence-based as any other notable scientific theory, some people show a strong reluctance to accept it. In this study, we used the Measure of Acceptance of the Theory of Evolution (MATE) and Knowledge of Evolution Exam (KEE) questionnaires with university students from four academic degree programs (Chemistry, English, History, and Biology) of ten universities from Spain to measure, respectively, acceptance and knowledge of evolutionary theory among third-year undergraduate students (nMATE = 978; nKEE = 981). Results show that acceptance of evolution is relatively high (87.2%), whereas knowledge of the theory is moderate (5.4 out of 10) although there are differences across degrees (Biology>Chemistry>History>English), and even among various universities (ranging from 4.71 to 5.81). Statistical analysis reveals that knowledge of evolutionary theory among Biology students is partially explained by the relative weight of evolutionary themes within the curriculum, suggesting that an increase in the number of hours dedicated to this topic could have a direct influence on students’ knowledge of it. We also found that religion may have a significant—although relatively small—negative influence on evolutionary theory acceptance. The moderate knowledge of evolution in our undergraduate students, together with the potential problem of acceptance in certain groups, suggests the need for a revision of the evolutionary concepts in the teaching curricula of our students since primary school.

Introduction

Evolution is a core theory of biology [1,2], although its influence extends to other disciplines such as philosophy [35], psychology [6], and medicine [7,8], as well as, in general, most life and social sciences, including politics and policy [911]. Evolution represents a well-established and mature theory that can explain how organisms evolve and differentiate from the last universal common ancestor (usually known as LUCA): a life form that originated on our planet about 3700 billion years ago [12]. Therefore, both the notion of descent with modifications and the theory of evolution represent key pieces of knowledge in our societies to understand both the world in which we live and, as well, our place in nature [13]. On the other hand, misconceptions about evolution commonly persist along the whole educational system, an effect that has been attributed to the possibility that evolution theory is counterintuitive [14], even working against our natural reasoning [15]. Nevertheless, and perhaps due to this key relevance and difficulty of understanding, the idea of evolution has been debated since its first formal proposal by Darwin [16,17]. A pioneer study led by Miller et al. [18] has shown that the level of evolutionary acceptance in citizens can vary considerably across countries and social groups. Therefore, research on the causes of such variations, as well as means to improve acceptance and knowledge levels, can be considered one of the main subtopics of evolution education [19].

There is lack of consensus in the field about the major determinants of evolution acceptance. For some authors, obstacles to accepting evolutionary theory show particularities that differ from those relating to other well-established scientific theories [20]. Such aspects have been attributed to the fact that certain intransigent religious views clash with evolutionary views, together with the variation in religiosity observed among countries or even social groups within a given country [2,21,22]. However, empirical studies [2325] typically have found three major factors determining evolution acceptance: religiosity, evolution understanding, and nature of science (NOS) understanding [26]. Therefore, a complex problem like this is most probably multifactorial while, at the same time, the different factors involved may presumably be partially correlated. For example, degree of religiosity is about equally correlated with evolution acceptance and two measures of education level in data from 34 countries [27]. The difficulty of determining factors leading to evolution acceptance has been suggested to hamper the derivation of optimal education strategies for our citizens [19]. In fact, it has been claimed that the level of evolution acceptance has remained low for the last 30 years in the USA [21].

One factor that could contribute to these disparate views is variability in the measures used to assess evolution acceptance: each can capture different evolution acceptance characteristics or even show differential levels of influence with other factors (reviewed in [19]). A variety of tests have been proposed to measure evolution acceptance, including the Measure of Acceptance of the Theory of Evolution (MATE) [28], the Evolutionary Attitudes and Literacy Survey (EALS) [29], the Inventory of Student Evolution Acceptance (I-SEA) [30], the Generalized Acceptance of EvolutioN Evaluation (GAENE) [31], the Attitudes toward Evolution (ATEVO) [32], etc. Although a consensus has not fully been achieved, the MATE seems to incorporate several of the ideal properties for this measurement [33]. The main advantage is that the MATE has been the most frequently-used test to date [19,21,34,35]. Moreover, when this instrument has been compared with alternatives, a moderate to high correlation between them has typically been observed [19,21,36,37].

The same lack of consensus exists when measuring the level of evolution knowledge, given that several alternative assessment tools are available: the Conceptual Inventory of Natural Selection (CINS) [38], Conceptual Assessment of Natural Selection (CANS) [39], Knowledge About Evolution (KAEVO) [32], Assessing Contextual Reasoning about Natural Selection (ACORNS) [40] and Knowledge of Evolution Exam (KEE) [41], among others. Another potential problem in studies trying to compare results of acceptance and knowledge about evolution across countries or social groups is the high heterogeneity of data available, as different studies focus on primary/secondary school, undergraduate, graduate, teachers or even variant social groups. Moreover, in certain cases, the questionnaires are offered online without any control of the time employed by each respondent. Therefore, any comparative study, in order to properly compare different experimental groups, should ideally use the same design on a homogeneous data set when possible.

Spain is considered an industrialized country (13th position in the World GDP ranking) [42] belonging to the European Union. Information about the knowledge and acceptance of evolution in Spain is scarce, although supposedly there is not any special social group reluctant to embrace evolution (showing intermediate to high ranking regarding evolution acceptance in Miller et al. [18]). Nevertheless, from within the educational field, it has been claimed that the curricula of primary and secondary school is rather poor in introducing evolutionary theory, at least compared to the most (educationally) advanced countries [43]. This fact could explain the subjective feeling of many university teachers that ideas on evolution are not in the students' backpack when they reach the university. With this potential problem in mind, we tried to check the prevalence of evolution acceptance and the knowledge level regarding this topic in undergraduate students attending the Spanish university system. We presented a three-section online questionnaire (incorporating items on demographics, evolution-acceptance, and evolution-knowledge) to third-year university students from several disciplines and institutions throughout Spain. The main objective was to characterize the level of acceptance and knowledge at different universities and degree programs and identify any group particularly reluctant to accept those ideas. A secondary objective was to check if the level of evolutionary theory in each degree program curriculum could explain the variation in evolutionary knowledge across universities.

Materials and methods

Bibliographic review of undergraduate evolution acceptance

One advantage of the MATE test is that it has been frequently used to infer the level of evolution acceptance from different countries (52 studies reviewed in Barnes et al. [19]). However, these studies had a high degree of individual diversity; further, we revised them to exclude all cases that did not represent exclusively undergraduate university students. In addition, in November 2019, we searched references in the WEB of Science database by applying the keywords “questionnaire” and “evolution”; in the list obtained, we used exclusively those conducted with the MATE and on undergraduate university students.

Participants

We scored certain demographic questions and evolution acceptance and knowledge tests administered to 1050 third-year students (n = 981 after excluding incomplete data) from four degree programs (Chemistry, History, English Philology, and Biology) at ten Spanish universities (Alicante, Autónoma de Madrid, Complutense de Madrid, Granada, Illes Balears, Salamanca, Santiago de Compostela, Sevilla, Valencia, and Vigo; Fig 1). Erasmus program students (foreign students funded by the European Union who came to study for one academic year) were identified and excluded from final analyses. We chose two science and two humanity degrees, having replicates within each group and assuming a priori that Biology students should have the highest rank in at least evolutionary knowledge (as it is the only subject where evolutionary concepts appear in the curriculum). We studied 10 of 21 possible public universities (awarding these four degrees) which could be considered as a representative sample for this country. The assessment was run during the 2018–2019 academic year, between September and December; the sample size per degree and university is shown in S1 Table.

thumbnail
Fig 1. Comparison of MATE and KEE across Degrees.

Bars are mean scores for MATE (in blue) and KEE (in red) for the different universities in the Spanish public university system included in the study and their geographic location. UVIGO (Universidade de Vigo), USC (Universidade de Santiago de Compostela), UAM (Universidad Autónoma de Madrid), UCM (Universidad Complutense de Madrid), UV (Universidad de Valencia), UIB (Universitat Illes Balears), UA (Universidad de Alicante), UGR (Universidad de Granada), US (Universidad de Sevilla) and USAL (Universidad de Salamanca).

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0238345.g001

Data collection

We asked for permission from the dean at each school and university to share our questionnaires with students. Usually, the dean suggested contacting a particular teacher of a third-year course from the school. Typically, one of the authors met with the class and explained the procedure to the students, including its voluntary and anonymous nature. Students were surveyed through a Google Forms quiz accessible through the Internet using their own electronic devices; they were given 15 minutes to finish the task (see S1 Table). The questionnaire was divided into three sections: personal-demographic questions, MATE-related questions and KEE-based questions (see next section), following the order above. A university-specific alphanumeric code was provided immediately before the start of the test; it was required in order to proceed and submit the form. We checked a posteriori that all questionnaires from a particular university and degree were completed on the exact dates and times, following notification from the coordinator.

The demographic questionnaire (see S1 Table) asked the sex of the participant (Sex; man, woman, I prefer not to choose), the age (Age), academic level attained (Academic level; secondary, graduated, doctor), secondary school itinerary (Itinerary; science, technology, humanities, social sciences, and art), degree that was being pursued (Degree; Chemistry, History, English, and Biology), university (University; the 10 schools described excluding Erasmus) and the religiosity level (Religiosity; usually attends religious services or not). The demographic questions are listed and summarized in various columns of the S1 Table.

Tests for evolution acceptance and knowledge

We used the MATE test [28] to assess evolution acceptance. This test is a 20-item Likert-type scale instrument with six subscales of measurement (process of evolution, scientific validity, human evolution, evidence, scientific community’s view, and age of the earth). This test has been considered to be internally consistent and having a high test-retest consistence [19], although certain authors have criticized it (see for example [34,35]). The original description reported only one factor, by factor analysis, which estimates evolution acceptance [28], but more recent analyses suggested that it could have two-dimensional constructs [34,36]: Facts and Credibility. Therefore, we calculated MATE scores following the original description and also using, separately, Facts and Credibility scores, following Romine et al. [34], to check whether using one or two dimensions could affect the results observed.

We chose the KEE test [41] to measure evolutionary knowledge. This is a simple test, a 10-item instrument. A few arguments support this choice. First, we needed a simple test that did not require much time during completion of the questionnaires, as we are asking teachers to use 15 minutes from a routine class. Moreover, we tried to avoid using two consecutive long tests in order to maximize student response. Furthermore, KEE and MATE had been chosen together as a practical combination in several studies [21,41,44].

Validation and controls

We estimated the reliability of MATE and KEE data by using Cronbach’s alpha statistics, as has been suggested to be done routinely for this kind of study [28,33,34]. In addition, we consider that both our religiosity and evolution knowledge (KEE) measures needed to independently validate whether they show a high correlation with alternative constructed measurements. An 8-item questionnaire with a Likert 1–5 scale on knowledge of the theory of evolution (CTE), built and validated by Cofré et al. [45], was used for validation. For the measurement of intrinsic religiosity -IR-, the DUREL test [46,47] was used (3 items with a Likert 1–5 scale). This was accomplished in a set of 268 students from 3 degrees of the University of Granada (n = 264 after eliminating incomplete responses). The sample is thus composed of 220 women (83.3%) and 44 men (16.7%), which is consistent with the sex distribution in the grades. The distribution by study areas was 171 students in Social education (64.8%), 33 in Archeology (12.5%), and 60 in Criminology (22.7%). The baccalaureate (high school) curriculum was mostly that of Social Sciences (53.0%), followed by humanities (22.0%), and sciences (16.0%), while those of technology (7.2%) and arts (2.6%) were in the minority. The academic level of the participants—prior to starting the degree—was Baccalaureate (89.4%) or Bachelor/Other degree (10.2%). Regarding religiosity, 82.8% of the participants said they were not religious, while 17.2% declared themselves religious, although only 2.6% indicated they were practicing (question about religiosity associated with the MATE / KEE questionnaires). The questionnaires were administered using pencil-and-paper and later stored on Excel® sheets.

We used an indirect method to infer the relative importance of NOS understanding on MATE/KEE scores by comparing the relative value of these scores on different degrees. A high level of acceptance associated with both Chemistry and Biology would suggest a NOS (understanding) factor contributing to evolution acceptance because these two degrees share a similar science itinerary in undergraduate and degree curriculum [48], while a primacy of Biology students over the rest of students would favor an interpretation based on evolutionary knowledge because this degree is the only one to have evolutionary knowledge in the degree curriculum (S2 Table).

Additionally, we checked the level of MATE and KEE in Biology teachers from two universities (Autónoma de Madrid and Vigo). This was considered as a positive control, because we expected that the teacher level, on average, would be higher than in corresponding third-year Biology students from their own universities. A few of the professors were instructing in other degrees (Chemistry, Mathematics, etc.).

Finally, we investigated the amount of evolutionary concepts described in the Biology curricula of the same ten Universities included in the study. The evolutionary subjects relative to the number of themes per lecture and University are described in S2 Table. These allowed us to estimate the contribution of evolutionary theory to the credits degree (Evolution Credits) as the sum of the lecture credits (Credits ECTS) weighted by percentage of evolutionary concepts (Probability of Evolution content). Notice that all Spanish public degrees in Biology show 240 ECTS credits.

Data analysis and statistics

The Google Form platform automatically generates a spreadsheet containing each participant answer in a different column from the same row. Using a homemade R script [49], we accessed each spreadsheet (one per university) and encoded the answers appropriately for downstream statistical analysis in csv files (data available at Figshare Repository; DOI:10.6084/m9.figshare.12623747). We merged all csv files and excluded questionnaires in which any student had not answered all items.

The associations among demographic variables were studied by a nonparametric chi-squared test. The probability was adjusted by the SGoF multitest procedure [50]. The main interest of the study was, however, to investigate the contribution of factors Religiosity (fixed), Degree (fixed), and University (nested within the interaction) into different dependent variables (MATE; MATE Facts, MATE Credibility and KEE scores) by a three-way ANOVA. This design allowed us to focus on the factors Religiosity and Degree, being tested against their interaction pooled with the nested factor (because the p-value of interaction was ≥ 0.25; [51]). This latter approach enabled us to detect if differences between levels of factors (Religiosity and Degree) persisted across the University variation [52]. The homoscedasticity of the data was evaluated by the Levine test. However, we used general linear models (with sum square type III), which are one of the most robust procedures to analyze data showing deviation from the parametric assumptions like normality or heteroscedasticity [53]. When the heteroscedasticity could not be corrected we checked whether the treatment with the most different mean showed the highest variance, the only dangerous heterocedasticity when interpreting ANOVA results [51].

The positive control on Biology teachers from two universities was accomplished by a two-way ANOVA on dependent variables (MATE and KEE) and factors University (fixed) and Religiosity (fixed). In this case, the correlation between Religiosity and MATE was accomplished within University. On the other hand, we also measured the correlation between the averaged Evolution themes within Biology curricula and the KEE score among universities. In all former cases, we used Pearson's r coefficient to estimate the degree of association, and the non-parametric Spearman r correlation to test its significance.

In addition, we used stepwise multiple linear regression to explain the evolution acceptance (MATE, MATE Facts, and MATE Credibility) by the potential exploratory variables available (Degree, Religiosity, Age, Level, and KEE scores). The variables Degree and University were recoded for this analysis following its averaged value for MATE (e.g. for Degree; English = 1; Chemistry = 2; History = 3; Biology = 4). We understand that such variables can confound many causes that explain their variation, but we wanted to check if they could obscure or not the other exploratory variables. The significance was checked by a regression ANOVA and the contribution of any variable entering into the model, independently of other variables, was estimated by the partial correlation coefficient [52]. All statistical analyses were done by the SPSS/PC software, version 24.0.0.0 [54], while the graphics were generated by the R project [49].

Results

Evolution acceptance review in the literature

We found data on MATE scores for undergraduate students in 6 countries (Table 1). The per-country average ranged from 55.5 in Pre-service education in Biology from Turkey to 84.2 in Germany.

thumbnail
Table 1. Review of MATE scores (mean ± SD) for undergraduate university students from different countries.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0238345.t001

Validation with different tests

Cronbach's alpha gave a high reliability value for the DUREL (IR) construct (0.797), while it was lower for the CTE evolutionary knowledge one (0.635). This represents a low reliability, although at acceptable levels. Regarding the sociodemographic variables, the test of independence between pairs of variables (Sex, Religiosity, Academic itinerary, and Degree) were significant with the chi-square test only in some pairs, e.g. Sex and Degree (2 of 8 tests) and Academic Itinerary and Degree (10 of 15 tests). It should be noted that the proportion of women in the degrees in which the questionnaires were applied was very high. Regarding knowledge of the theory, the application of the CTE test yielded an average score of 26.53 (n = 264; Min. 18; Max. 40; SD = 3.225) which, adjusted to the range of the KEE scale for comparison, indicates a value of M = 3.16 ± 0.5. This can be considered close to that obtained in English History and Philology degrees, if we take into account the random noise in the case of KEE scores.

The data obtained when applying the DUREL indicated that just over two-thirds of the participants (69.7%) did not consider themselves religious people. The scores of the level of intrinsic religiosity through the DUREL test (with Likert scale 1–5) indicated that the degree of religiosity was low (Mean DUREL ± SD = 2.02 ± 0.935; n = 264). This finding could be important to consider when comparing this test with knowledge data. Disaggregating the three items of the IR test, the religious experience turned out to be at a medium low level (DUREL = 2.34 ± 0.935) and fell still lower when respondents were asked about whether religious beliefs defined their vision of life (DUREL = 1.99 ± 1.052), and even more so when asked about trying to bring religion to other matters of life (DUREL = 1.73 ± 0.990).

We compared the DUREL IR and level of knowledge of the theory of evolution (CTE), applying a nonparametric correlation test. A moderate negative correlation coefficient (rho = -0.124; n = 264; P = 0.045) was obtained.

Analyses of questionnaires

The descriptive frequencies and scores for the different demographic variables are shown in S1 Table. Pairs of different variables (Sex, Religiosity, Academic level, Itinerary, and Degree) per university were usually nonsignificant by the chi-square test (15 significant out of 100 tests; S3 Table). Most significant cases affected a few pairs of variables: Sex by Degree (4 of 10 tests) and Itinerary by Degree (10 of 10 tests). The frequency of treatments for demographic variables was always heterogeneous among Universities (S4 Table). This typically occurred because just one University departed from the general trend (as in Sex, Degree and Itinerary), but in two variables (Degree and Religiosity) no clear general trend was found.

Our data showed a high reliability of the MATE test (Cronbach’s r = 0.87; n = 978; items = 20) but lower reliability for the KEE test (Cronbach’s r = 0.42; n = 981; items = 10), indicating a safe inference at least in the MATE analyses. Results from the multifactorial ANOVAs for different dependent variables are presented in Table 2. The MATE was heteroscedastic; however, none of the transformations used allowed for correcting this; we therefore present untransformed data analyses. Data derived on this instrument showed significant differences for Religiosity, Degree, and University (nested factor). Heteroscedasticity did not compromise the p-values for Degree, because the treatment that differed from the rest (Biology) showed the smallest variance. However, it could potentially compromise the p-value for Religiosity, as this treatment showed a rather high variance. So our mean differences in MATE between Religiosity treatments could be partially caused by variance differences. Interestingly, the interaction between Religiosity and Degree was not significant, suggesting that these two factors independently contribute to acceptance of evolution. The two-dimensional scores of the MATE (MATE Facts and MATE Credibility) showed the same trend and pattern of significance as the MATE (Table 2). Actually, facts and credibility showed a rather high correlation between them (rPearson = 0.71; n = 978; PSpearman < 0.001). The results for the MATE are graphically shown in Fig 2: a high averaged evolution acceptance > 80 was observed for all degrees (mean ± SD = 87.2 ± 8.63). The figure also shows how religious individuals tend to appear in the region of lower MATE values but with a large variance in the responses.

thumbnail
Fig 2. Violin plots for MATE per Degree.

The individual Religiosity score of every individual is presented. MATE scores per individual are represented with dots, coloured based on their correspondent religious scores. Each violin consists of two symmetric vertical smoothed probability densities (left and right curves). Inside each violin plot there is a boxplot conformed by one narrow grey rectangle ranging from the first to the third quartile (Q1 to Q3) of the distribution and two thin lines (upper and lower whiskers) extending from Q3 to Q3 + 1.5 × (Q3—Q1) and from Q1 to Q1–1.5 × (Q3- Q1). Mean values are shown with black diamonds. Dots beyond whiskers are outliers.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0238345.g002

thumbnail
Table 2. Three-way ANOVA (Religiosity, Degree and University nested within the interaction) for different dependent variables about evolution acceptance and knowledge.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0238345.t002

The data were homoscedastic for the KEE; we therefore present untransformed analyses. Only the factor Degree was significant for the KEE (Table 2). The differences were in the order of Biology (mean ± SD for KEE = 6.5 ± 1.71) > Chemistry (5.2 ± 1.73) > History (4.8 ± 1.81) and English Philology (4.4 ± 1.66). However, the SNK test showed that only Biology differed significantly from the other treatments. Furthermore, when we studied the KEE scores exclusively within Biology, there were significant differences among Universities (F = 2.74; DF1 = 9; DF2 = 344; P = 0.004). Moreover, in that case there was a significant correlation between the Evolution Credits and KEE score among the ten studied Universities (r = 0.567; n = 10; PSpearman = 0.011).

A detailed analysis of evolution knowledge in Biology on the KEE items revealed three different patterns across Faculties (Fig 3). One item (K2) showed a similarly high rate of response among students of any degree, while several items (K5, K7, K8 and K9) showed similar but low scores. Finally, another group (K1, K3, K4, K6 and K10) showed the expected variations among different undergraduate students, with the highest score for those studying Biology.

thumbnail
Fig 3. Average values of correct answer per KEE item, grouped in three different classes: Most guessed (K2), most failed (K5, K7, K8 and K9) and differences between Faculties (K1, K3, K4, K6 and K10).

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0238345.g003

A positive control of our analysis was done on the acceptance and knowledge levels of Biology teachers from two universities (Vigo and Autónoma de Madrid). In this case, the two-way ANOVA did not detect any significant factor either for the MATE or the KEE, indicating that teacher evolution acceptance and knowledge may not differ across levels of Degree and Religiosity. These dependent variables showed that teachers had a higher average than students: mean ± SD for MATE (93.6 ± 6.22; n = 50) and KEE (7.6 ± 1.86; n = 53). The correlation between Religiosity and MATE was not significant (PVigo = 0.085; PMadrid = 0.965).

Finally, we used multiple regression analysis to explore which of the available variables (Age, Religiosity, KEE score, Degree, and University) could explain evolution acceptance (MATE, MATE Facts, and MATE Credibility; Table 3). The results show similar regression models under any of the acceptance scores used; in all cases, the order of variables entered into the model were KEE (evolution knowledge), followed by Religiosity, University, and Degree. Moreover, partial correlation coefficients were rather similar to standardized regression coefficients (not shown), which indicates that these variables do in fact contribute independently to the explanation of evolution acceptance.

thumbnail
Table 3. Stepwise multiple regression of different exploratory variables (see text) to predict the different dependent variables (evolution acceptance and knowledge measures).

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0238345.t003

Discussion

Evolutionary theory acceptance may depend on factors such as religiosity, evolution knowledge, and NOS understanding, although their relative contribution is a contentious issue [2325]. Part of the problem is the existence of confounding factors that do not allow a focus on the actual cause (e.g. use of a range of questionnaires, often with varying structures and/or properties or the fact that different studies have been applied to different social groups). Here we have tried to focus on a well-established evolution-acceptance tool (MATE) and studied exclusively third-year students from the same four degrees at ten public Spanish Universities. This strategy may reduce the intrinsic variability of responses and allow detection of factors with relatively small contributions, compared to other studies. In relation to this, our demographic analysis suggests that only two pairs of variables were dependent: Sex by Degree and Itinerary by Degree. The first pair of variables is beyond the scope of this study, although there may exist several explanations that can influence differential academic interest in males and females (see an extensive review in Halpern [65]). The second pair of variables is an absolutely expected relationship, because pre-university itineraries are supposed to prepare the students for final degree choices. Therefore, most biology and chemistry students come from a high school itinerary in Science, while the situation is more variable in the other two degrees (although most students come from undergraduate humanities and social science itineraries).

Evolution acceptance has been considered a complex parameter to estimate. Indeed, the MATE questionnaire has been suggested by some authors [34,36] to include two constructs: Facts and Credibility. However, our separate analysis of the two construct estimators rendered similar results as were obtained with the original MATE—a result observed in other studies [34,36] as well. Therefore, irrespective of the true dimensionality of MATE, we can consider it as a valid estimate of evolution acceptance, at least in our data set.

The analysis of Spanish university students reveals that levels of evolution acceptance are relatively high (average range 81.2–92.8 per university), at the same level as most modern industrialized countries (Table 1). Actually, biology teachers from two universities obtained a higher average than their corresponding students. Interestingly, we observed differences in student’s evolution acceptance for Degree and Religiosity but not for their interaction. These observations suggest that these two factors may provide an independent contribution to the level of evolution acceptance. Actually, the same result was observed when searching an explicative model by multiple regression. We can discuss the contribution of these two factors separately.

Religiosity has been suggested to be a major contributor to evolution acceptance in several studies [18,21,35,66]. Although our religiosity estimate is somehow simpler than other published alternatives, our validation experiment confirms that the more detailed construct of the DUREL instrument rendered similar tendencies as in the general study. However, one clear limitation in our design is that we asked for religiosity irrespective of the type of religion. It could be that specific theistic views may confront evolution beliefs more directly [44]. This is something that should be corrected in future studies. It also could explain why religiosity had a relatively small influence on evolution acceptance in our data set (6.6% reduction) compared to other studies (Table 1). Moreover, in our investigation, we also detected the highest variation for MATE in the high religiosity subgroup—an association that could be explained if individuals belonging to both theist and non-theist religions could perhaps differentially contribute to it. The association of religiosity and MATE is quite variable across countries. For example, in the USA, people with higher religiosity levels can reduce its acceptance by up to 20–50% [18,41], although such reduction is typically less than 20% in biology undergraduate students (Table 1). Moreover, evolution acceptance itself may depend on education level, as in-service teachers from the USA increase levels of acceptance to 85.0 ± 4.5 [67,68]. Interestingly, the two countries with the lowest acceptance levels belong to countries with a majority of theist religious citizens, suggesting that these kinds of religions present the greatest a priori prejudices toward evolutionary views (Table 1). It is worth noting most controversies about these factors pertain to whether they represent a true causative factor or a correlated effect (as if high religiosity students show either low evolution knowledge or NOS levels; see Rice et al. [44]). This controversy can be partially data-dependent; thus, in certain social contexts religiosity may be a main component by itself while, in others, it could rather be indirectly mediated by other factors. Notice, however, that among Spanish university students the contribution of religiosity was relatively low but apparently independent of evolution knowledge (KEE test) or NOS level (Degree). Other authors have already shown that the factors affecting evolution acceptance differ from those contributing to the acceptance of other scientific theories [20]. In summary, a society should be warned that certain religiosities may add some prejudices to any first encounter with evolutionary theory. Perhaps our scientific community should increase efforts to show that such a negative relationship between evolution and religion is neither natural nor justified [6971] or take an active predisposition to change it (e.g. Truong et al. [72]).

In our study, evolution acceptance also depended on the degree studied and, under the regression model, evolution knowledge. These considerations point again to the same major forces previously detected (Religiosity, Knowledge, and NOS understanding) [2325]. Actually, the three are directly compared in the regression model; such an analysis suggests (comparing partial correlation coefficients from Table 3) that in our study the ranking of factors may be Knowledge > Religiosity > NOS. Of course, such positions may depend on the particular set of citizens studied. As we used as participants third-year university students with presumably relatively high levels of NOS, this may have influenced the results. Actually, if NOS understanding were very important, we would expect that both Biology and Chemistry students (Science itinerary) would distance themselves from History and Philology (Social Sciences and Humanities Itineraries) while, if evolution knowledge is the main driver, we would expect that Biology would succeed in isolation (as the only one with actual evolution themes). Therefore, our results suggest that, at least in our university students, evolution knowledge may be more relevant than NOS understanding.

A different trend was observed when evolution knowledge was evaluated by the KEE questionnaire. The analysis showed homoscedasticity for this variable across treatments; thus, all the statistical inferences were very safe. On the other hand, we found rather low levels of reliability for this test (which suggests low-quality data for KEE). This represents a limitation in our ability to interpret the evolutionary knowledge, as we may not truly understand the relationship between knowledge and acceptance given that knowledge is indeed so low. However, we believe that a relatively high proportion of individuals may answer most questions randomly (as they do not know the answer and we asked the students to answer all items from KEE). This fact may be responsible for the low reliability value observed. Indeed, this is not dependent on the item studied, as we observed the same reliability using the one-item out of the analysis strategy (not shown). For KEE, only the Degree factor was significant (Biology students behave better than the rest; Fig 4), suggesting that it is the amount of evolution materials in the curricula that is the most relevant factor. We have another argument that points in the same direction, the differences among universities in evolution knowledge within Biology were partially explained (up to 30%) by degree of evolutionary themes in the Biology curricula. The Evolution Knowledge observed in our students was within the range of those detected in previous studies [21,41,44]. However, in those earlier investigations, there was a relatively high variability among different social subgroups. For example, students who received the first instruction on evolution within a religious environment consistently presented a lower evolution knowledge level (3.2) than those who had received such instruction in the high school setting (5.4; scale modified from Moore et al. [41]). On the other hand, as expected, evolution teachers from the two tested universities showed a higher knowledge level than corresponding students. In summary, in our case, all results suggested that, to improve evolution knowledge, we must introduce more evolution-related materials into the curricula. This is a well-known problem in the Spanish education system, as there is very little content on evolutionary biology in primary and secondary school curricula. Indeed, evolutionary biology contents are only available to students who choose the Science Itinerary [43]. This is something the education community should try to change in the coming years.

thumbnail
Fig 4. Violin plots for KEE per Degree.

Violin plots and boxplots as explained in Fig 2. Here, mean values are shown with black squares and the interquartile range in blue.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0238345.g004

The study of responses for separated items for evolutionary knowledge gave different results. Item 2 was correctly answered by most students (regarding reasons for pesticide resistance; S1 Fig), while items about the level of natural selection (5 and 7), description of macroevolution (8), and the ultimate source of variation (9) were poorly answered. The positive response to Item 2 can be interpreted to mean that most students are aware of some of the practical implications that evolutionary theory has on the living world. The items answered negatively by most students could be considered a potential guide to the kind of evolutionary concepts that are hard for the students to grasp. Other authors have focused on the differential responses item by item [40,73], although the differential tool considered may condition the result. For example, [73] Nehm and Ha [73] found that students gave more frequent correct responses under trait gain than loss, a bias that cannot be checked by the KEE.

In summary, we have observed relatively high levels of evolution theory acceptance using the MATE in third-year university students attending ten Spanish Universities. The main factors that seemed to influence levels of acceptance were Degree > Religiosity > KEE, which suggests that both religiosity and evolution knowledge are relevant and partially independent factors to be considered. A similar strategy should be accomplished in different segments of the educational course, for example, just prior to entry into the degree program or immediately after finishing it. Moreover, although the reduction in acceptance of evolution due to religiosity is relatively low, we should consider strategies to diminish the a priori prejudice against the concept, as held by highly religious individuals. Perhaps a general effort to persuade the religious part of the society that religion and evolution are not necessarily antagonistic views could be useful as well. A more serious set of limitations exists regarding evolution knowledge which, in general, has been relatively low in all grades, including biology. This suggests an obvious strategy for improving this situation, i.e. to incorporate more evolutionary theory in both pre-university levels, affecting all students, and in the university, affecting particularly the curricula in Biology and other life sciences study programs.

Supporting information

S2 Data. University questionaries teache.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0238345.s002

(XLSX)

S1 Table. Descriptive parameters of the data questionnaires.

N is number of students, Nanalysis is the number used in the study (after excluding uncomplete cases). Religiosity represents the percentage of students that claim to be religious practitioner.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0238345.s003

(DOCX)

S2 Table. Description of the curricula of the Biology degrees per University.

Course is the year of the given lecture. Total content is the number of themes of the lecture, while subject content is the number of those related with Evolution. Probability is the ratio between the former two, while Evolution credits is the sum of the credits x probability.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0238345.s004

(DOCX)

S3 Table. Pairwise chi-square tests for the different variables pairs per university.

Pink shaded for those significant (without multitest correction) and Red shaded for those significant after SGoF multitest adjustment.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0238345.s005

(DOCX)

S4 Table. Analysis of the homogeneity chi-square test for demographic variables within University.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0238345.s006

(DOCX)

Acknowledgments

We thank all participating students as well as the Faculty Deans and teachers who helped during presentation of the online questionnaires to the students from the ten Spanish Universities. We thank the Spanish Society of Evolutionary Biology (SESBE), which allowed us to use its member list to search for participants from various Spanish regions. All authors declare they have no conflict of interest.

References

  1. 1. Dobzhansky T. Nothing in biology makes sense except in the light of evolution. Am Biology Teach. 1973;35: 125–129.
  2. 2. Coyne JA. Why evolution is true. New York: Viking Books; 2009.
  3. 3. Ruse M. Taking Darwin seriously: A naturalistic approach to philosophy. Oxford: Basil Blackwell; 1985.
  4. 4. Dennett DC. Darwin’s dangerous idea: Evolution and the meanings of life. New York: Simon & Schuster, 1995.
  5. 5. Stamos D. N. (2008). Evolution and the Big Questions: Sex, Race, Religion, and Other Matters. Hoboken, NJ: Wiley-Blackwell.
  6. 6. Buss DM. Evolutionary Psychology. 5th ed. London: Routledge; 2015.
  7. 7. Williams GC, Nesse RM. The dawn of darwinian medicine. Q Rev Biol. 1991;66: 1–22. pmid:2052670
  8. 8. Perlman RL (2013). Evolution and Medicine. Oxford: Oxford University Press; 2013.
  9. 9. Singer P. A darwinian left: Politics, evolution and cooperation. London: Weidenfeld & Nicholson; 1999.
  10. 10. Rosenberg A. Darwinism in philosophy, social science and policy. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press; 2000.
  11. 11. Dupré J. What the theory of evolution can’t tell us. Crit Q. 2000;42: 18–34.
  12. 12. Pearce BKD, Tupper AS, Pudritz RE,Higgs PG. Constraining the time interval for the origin of life on earth. Astrobiology. 2018;18: 1–22.
  13. 13. Dupré J. Darwin’s legacy: What evolution means today. Oxford: Oxford University Press; 2005.
  14. 14. Dubochet J. Why is it so difficult to accept Darwin’s theory of evolution? Bioessays. 2011;33: 240–242. pmid:21254153
  15. 15. Kelemen D. Teleological minds: How natural intuition about agency and purpose influence learning about evolution. In: Rosengreen KS, Brem SK, Evans EM, Sinatra GM, editors. Evolution challenges. Oxford: Oxford University Press; 2012. pp. 66–92.
  16. 16. Hull DL. Darwin and his critics: The reception of Darwin’s theory of evolution by the scientific community. Chicago: The University of Chicago Press; 1983.
  17. 17. Bowler PJ. Evolution: The History of an Idea. 3rd ed. Berkeley: University of California Press; 2003.
  18. 18. Miller JD, Scott EC, Okamoto S. Public acceptance of evolution. Science. 2006;313: 765–766. pmid:16902112
  19. 19. Barnes ME, Dunlop HM, Holt EA, Zheng Y, Brownell SE. (2019). Different evolution acceptance instruments lead to different research findings. Evo Edu Outreach. 2019;12: 4.
  20. 20. Rutledge ML, Sadler KC. University students’ acceptance of biological theories—Is evolution really different? J Coll Sci Teach. 2011;41: 38–43.
  21. 21. Rissler LJ, Duncan SI, Caruso NM. The relative importance of religion and education on the university’s students views of evolution in the Deep South and state science standards across the United States. Evo Edu Outreach. 2014;7: 24.
  22. 22. Gallup [Internet]. In US, belief in creationist view of humans at new low [cited 2020 Jul 3]. Avaliable from: http://www.gallup.com/poll/210956/belief-creationist-view-humans-new-low.aspx Accessed 11 november 2019
  23. 23. Glaze AL, Goldston MJ, Dantzler J. Evolution in the southeastern USA: factors influencing acceptance and rejection in pre-service science teachers. Int J Sci Math Educ. 2014;13: 1189–1209.
  24. 24. Dunk RD, Petto AJ, Wiles JR, Campbell BC. A multifactorial analysis of acceptance of evolution. Evo Edu Outreach. 2017;10: 4.
  25. 25. Mead R, Hejmadi M, Hurst LD. Scientific aptitude better explains poor responses to teaching of evolution than psychological conflicts. Nat Ecol Evol. 2018;2: 388–394. pmid:29311699
  26. 26. McComas WF. Understanding how science works: The nature of science as the foundation for science teaching and learning. Sch Sci Rev. 2017;98: 71–76.
  27. 27. Heddy BC, Nadelson LS. A global perspective of the variables associated with acceptance of evolution. Evo Edu Outreach. 2012;6: 3.
  28. 28. Rutledge ML, Warden MA. The development and validation of the measure of acceptance of the theory of evolution instrument. Sch Sci Math. 1999;99: 13–8.
  29. 29. Hawley PH, Short SD, McCune LA, Osman MR, Little TD. What’s the matter with Kansas? The development and confirmation of the Evolutionary Attitudes and Literacy Survey (EALS). Evo Edu Outreach. 2011;4: 117–132.
  30. 30. Nadelson LS, Southerland SA. Examining the interaction of acceptance and understanding: How does the relationship change with a focus on macroevolution? Evo Edu Outreach. 2012;3: 82–88.
  31. 31. Smith MU, Snyder SW, Deveraux RS. The GAENE—Generalized Acceptance of EvolutioN Evaluation: Development of a new measure of Evolution acceptance. J Res Sci Teach. 2016;53: 1289–1315.
  32. 32. Beniermann A. [Evolution–of acceptance and doubts. Empirical studies on attitudes towards evolution and the evolution of the human mind]. Wiesbaden: Springer Spektrum; 2019. Available from: https://www.springer.com/de/book/9783658241049. German.
  33. 33. Mead LS, Kohn C, Warwick A, Schwartz K. Applying measurement standards to evolution education assessment instruments. Evo Edu Outreach. 2019;12: 5.
  34. 34. Romine WL, Walter EM, Bosse E, Todd AN. Understanding patterns of evolution acceptance–a new implementation of the Measure of Acceptance of the Theory of Evolution (MATE) with Midwestern university students. J Res Sci Teach. 2017;54: 642–671.
  35. 35. Metzger KJ, Motplaisir D, Haines D, Nickodem DK. Investigating undergraduate health sciences students’ acceptance of evolution using MATE and GAENE. Evo Edu Outreach. 2018;11: 10.
  36. 36. Romine WL, Todd AN, Walter EM. A closer look at the items within three measures of evolution acceptance: analysis of the MATE, I-SEA, and GAENE as a single corpus of items. Evo Edu Outreach. 2018;11: 17.
  37. 37. Sbeglia GC, Nehm RH. Measuring evolution acceptance using the GAENE: Influences of gender, race, degree-plan, and instruction. Evo Edu Outreach. 2018;11: 18.
  38. 38. Anderson DL, Fisher KM, Norman GJ. Development and evaluation of the conceptual inventory of natural selection. J Res Sci Teach. 2002;39: 952–78.
  39. 39. Kalinowski ST, Leonard MJ, Taper ML. Development and validation of the Conceptual Assessment of Natural Selection (CANS). CBE Life Sci Educ. 2016;15(4): ar64. pmid:27856552
  40. 40. Nehm RH, Beggrow EP, Opfer JE, Ha M. Reasoning about natural selection: Diagnosing contextual competency using ACORNS instrument. Am Biology Teach. 2012;74: 92–98.
  41. 41. Moore R, Cotner S, Bates A. The influence of religion and high school biology courses on students’ knowledge of evolution when they enter college. J Effect Teach. 2009;9: 4–12.
  42. 42. International Monetary Fund World Economic Outlook [Internet]. Projected GDP Ranking (2019–2024) [cited 2020 Jul 3]. Available from: http://statisticstimes.com/economy/projected-world-gdp-ranking.php
  43. 43. Vázquez-Ben L, Bugallo-Rodríguez Á. The model of biological evolution in the curriculum of Primary Education: an international comparative analysis. Rev Eureka Ensen Divulg Cienc. 2018;15(3): 3101. Spanish.
  44. 44. Rice JW, Clough MP, Olson JK, Adams DC, Colbert JT. University faculty and their knowledge & acceptance of biological evolution. Evo Edu Outreach. 2015;8: 8.
  45. 45. Cofré H. L., Vergara C. A., Santibáñez D. P., & Jiménez J. P. (2013). A first approach to the understanding of the Theory of Evolution by Chilean university students. Estudios Pedagógicos. 2013;39: 67–83. Spanish.
  46. 46. Koenig H, Parkerson GRJ, Meador KG. Religion index for psychiatric research. Am J Psychiatry. 1997;154(6): 885–886.
  47. 47. Koenig HG, Büssing A. The Duke University Religion Index (DUREL): A five-item measure for use in epidemiological studies. Religions. 2010;1(1): 78–85.
  48. 48. Ministerio de Educación y Formación Profesional [Internet] Madrid: Organización del Bachillerato; c2020 [cited 2020 Jul 3]. Available from: http://www.educacionyfp.gob.es/dam/jcr:95f179e5-f089-4cc6-a590-283cef703f19/02-tabla-organizaci-n-bachillerato.pdf
  49. 49. R Core Team. R: A language and environment for statistical computing [Internet]. Viena, Austria; 2016. Available from: http://www.R-project.org/
  50. 50. Carvajal-Rodríguez A, Uña-Alvarez J de, Rolán-Alvarez E. A new multitest correction (SGoF) that increases its statistical power when increasing the number of tests. BMC Bioinformatics. 2009;10: 209. pmid:19586526
  51. 51. Underwood AJ. Techniques of analysis of variance in experimental marine biology and ecology. Oceanogr Mar Biol. 1981;19: 513–605.
  52. 52. Sokal R, Rohlf FJ. Biometry: The principles and practice of statistics in biological research. 3rd ed. New York: W. H. Freeman and Company; 1995.
  53. 53. Mardia KV. The effect of nonnormality on some multivariate tests and robustness to nonnormality in the linear model. Biometrika. 1971;58: 105–121.
  54. 54. IBM Corp. IBM SPSS Statistics for Windows. Armonk: IBM Corp; 2017.
  55. 55. Großschedl J, Konnemann C, Basel N. Pre-service biology teachers’ acceptance of evolutionary theory and their preference for its teaching. Evo Edu Outreach. 2014;7: 1–16.
  56. 56. Athanasiou K, Papadopoulou P. Conceptual ecology of the evolution acceptance among Greek education students: knowledge, religious practices and social influences. Int J Sci Educ. 2012;34: 903–924.
  57. 57. Yousuf A, bin Daud MA, Nadeem A. Awareness and acceptance of evolution and evolutionary medicine among medical students in Pakistan. Evo Edu Outreach. 2011;4: 580–588.
  58. 58. Ha M, Haury DL, Nehm RH. Feeling of certainty: uncovering a missing link between knowledge and acceptance of evolution. J Res Sci Teach. 2012;49: 95–121.
  59. 59. Deniz H, Donnelly LA, Yilmaz I. Exploring the factor related to acceptance of evolutionary theory among Turkish preservice biology teachers: toward a more informative conceptual ecology for biological evolution. J Res Sci Teach. 2008; 45: 420–443.
  60. 60. Deniz H, Ҫetin F, Yilmaz I. (2011). Examining the relationships among acceptance of evolution, religiosity, and teaching preference for evolution in Turkish preservice Biology teachers. RNCSE. 2011;31: 1–9.
  61. 61. Deniz H, Sahin EA. Exploring the factors related to acceptance of evolutionary theory among Turkish preservice biology teachers and the relationship between acceptance and teaching preference. Electron J Sci Edu. 2016;20: 21–42.
  62. 62. Walter EM, Halverson KM, Boyce CJ. Investigating the relationship between college students’ acceptance of evolution and tree thinking understanding. Evo Edu Outreach. 2013;6: 26.
  63. 63. Cavallo AML, McCall D. (2008). Seeing may not mean believing: Examining students’ understanding and beliefs in Evolution. Am Biology Teach. 2008;70: 522–527.
  64. 64. Manwaring KF, Jensen JL, Gill RA, Bybee SM. Influencing highly religious undergraduate perceptions of evolution: mormons as a case study. Evo Edu Outreach. 2015;8: 23
  65. 65. Halpern DF. Sex differences in cognitive abilities. New York: Psychology Press; 2012.
  66. 66. Moore R, Brooks DC, Cotner S (2011). The relation of high school biology courses and students’ religious beliefs to college students’ knowledge of evolution. Am Biology Teach. 2011;73: 222–226.
  67. 67. Nadelson LS, Sinatra GM. Educational professionals’ knowledge and acceptance of evolution. Evol Psychol. 2009;7: 490–516.
  68. 68. Ha M, Baldwin BC, Nehm RH. The long-term impacts of short-term professional development: science teachers and evolution. Evo Edu Outreach. 2015;8: 11.
  69. 69. Ruse M. Can a Darwinian be a christian? Cambridge: Cambridge University Press; 2000.
  70. 70. National Academy of Sciences. Science, evolution, and creationism. Washington, DC: National Academies Press; 2008.
  71. 71. Sober E. Evolution without naturalism. In: Kvanvig JL, editor. Oxford Studies in Philosophy of Religion: Volume 3. Oxford: Oxford University Press; 2011. pp. 187–221.
  72. 72. Truong JM, Barnes ME, Brownell SE. Can six minutes of culturally competent evolution education reduce students' level of perceived conflict between evolution and religion? Am Biology Teach. 2018;80: 106–115.
  73. 73. Nehm RH, Ha M. Item feature effects in evolution assessment. J Res Sci Teach. 2011;48: 237–256.