Abstract
Background
To date services for children with Developmental Coordination Disorder (DCD) have not been informed by the perspective of children with DCD. This study aimed to synthesise the findings of discrete qualitative studies reporting the lived experiences views and preferences of children and young with DCD using a meta-ethnographic approach to develop new conceptual understandings.
Methods
A systematic search of ten databases; Academic Search Complete, AMED, CINAHL, ERIC, MEDLINE, PsychArticles, PsychInfo, EMBASE, SPORTDiscus, and Web of Science, was conducted between March and April 2019, and updated in early June 2020. Meta-ethnography, following the method described by Noblit and Hare was used to synthesise included studies. The Joanna Briggs Institute Checklist was used to appraise all included papers. PROSPERO registration number CRD42019129178.
Results
Fifteen studies met the inclusion criteria. Meta-ethnographic synthesis produced three themes; a) ‘It’s harder than it should be’: Navigating daily activities b) Fitting in, and c) ‘So what? I drop things’: Strategies and supports to mitigate challenges. Children with DCD describe a mismatch between their abilities and performance norms for daily activities that led to a cascade of negative consequences including negative self-appraisal, bullying and exclusion. In the face of these difficulties children described creative and successful strategies they enacted and supports they accessed including; assistance from others (parents, friends and teachers), focusing on their strengths and talents, accepting and embracing their difference, adopting a “just do it” attitude, setting personal goals, self-exclusion from some social activities, using humour or sarcasm, viewing performance expectations as a social construct, and enjoying friendships as a forum for fun, acceptance and protection against exclusion.
Conclusion
Service provision for children and young people with DCD should address the social and attitudinal environments, focus on friendship and social inclusion and address stigma-based bullying particularly within the school environment. Furthermore, practitioners should identify and foster children’s own strategies for navigating daily life activities with DCD. The identified themes resonate with contemporary disability theory and the International Classification of Functioning. The social and attitudinal environmental context of children and young people with DCD profoundly influences their experiences. Future intervention development and service provision for children and young people with DCD should consider opportunities to address social and attitudinal environmental factors.
Figures
Citation: O’Dea Á, Stanley M, Coote S, Robinson K (2021) Children and young people’s experiences of living with developmental coordination disorder/dyspraxia: A systematic review and meta-ethnography of qualitative research. PLoS ONE 16(3): e0245738. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0245738
Editor: Andrew Soundy, University of Birmingham, UNITED KINGDOM
Received: July 15, 2020; Accepted: January 6, 2021; Published: March 4, 2021
Copyright: © 2021 O’Dea et al. This is an open access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License, which permits unrestricted use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original author and source are credited.
Data Availability: All relevant data are within the paper and its Supporting Information files.
Funding: The authors disclosed receipt of the following financial support for the research, authorship and/or publication of this article: The author ÁOD disclosed that she is a full time PhD scholar on a structured PhD programme in health service research and population health, funded by the Health Research Board (SPHeRE/2013/1), Ireland. This programme funds postgraduate fees and a stipend for four years.
Competing interests: No authors have competing interests.
Introduction
This neurodevelopmental disorder Developmental Coordination Disorder (DCD) affects between 5% to 6% of the paediatric population [1] and is characterised by impaired motor proficiency, which interferes with the performance of activities of daily life, academic/school-based activities, leisure and play [1]. For a diagnosis of DCD, these motor proficiency difficulties cannot be explained by other neurological conditions that affect movement [1]. DCD is associated with an array of physical health problems [2], psychosocial and mental health problems [3, 4], and learning difficulties such as reading, social skills and inattention [5]. It is well established that DCD has the potential to impact on individuals involvement and participation in daily life activities and these challenges persist beyond childhood [6].
Best practice recommendations advocate that intervention planning for children with DCD should commence with an analysis of the individual’s strengths and weaknesses in their environmental context, so that an activity or participation-oriented approach can implemented [6]. However, there is a paucity of robust empirical evidence to guide the implementation of activity or participation-oriented practice with the current available evidence hampered by a wide range of methodological issues including limited controlled trials, poor outcome measurement, and intervention design [7, 8]. Therefore, there is a pressing need to develop interventions for children and adolescents with DCD [7, 8]. It is vital that future intervention development for children and young people with DCD is informed by stakeholder perspectives in line with recommendations for complex intervention development [9].
Children with disabilities have the right to be heard on issues that affect them [10], yet research on children’s experiences has tended to ignore the views of children as active agents and ‘key informants’ in matters pertaining to their health and wellbeing [11, 12]. Similarly, there has been a lack of attention to the perspectives of children with DCD in research, service developments and policy to date. For example the recently published ’International clinical practice recommendations on the definition, diagnosis, assessment, intervention, and psychosocial aspects of developmental coordination disorder’ [6], included stakeholder representation from a parent organisation for individuals with learning disorders, but no contribution from children or young people with DCD.
Multiple researchers have emphasised the value of research on children’s experiences and perspectives where children are acknowledged as authorities on their own lives [13–15]. Historically there has been a greater focus on eliciting the perspective of parents of children with DCD rather than the perspectives of children and adolescents with DCD. A number of studies have explored parents perspectives of their child receiving a diagnosis of DCD [16], parenting a child with DCD [17–20] and parental perspectives on their child with DCD’s reduced participation patterns across activities of daily living (ADL) [21, 22], out of school activities [23], leisure-time activity participation [24], and social participation [25]. It is important to note that the perspectives of parents, educators and allied health professionals can differ from those of children with a developmental disability [26–28].
Qualitative research can illuminate the meaning of everyday experiences [29] and reveal the perspectives, views and experiences of children and young people [30]. The past fifteen years has seen a growing number of qualitative studies focusing on children and adolescent’s experiences of living with DCD [31–34], including studies on; the importance of identity and empowerment to teenagers [31], children’s perceptions of the impact of DCD on activities of daily living [32], their perceptions of participation across home and community environments [33] and quality of life [34].
Although several studies examining various aspects of the experiences of children with DCD exist, as far as we are aware, no qualitative evidence synthesis has examined the totality of research on this topic. Meta-ethnographic synthesis is a popularly employed approach to qualitative evidence synthesis because it is allows researchers to bring together multiple qualitative studies, compare accounts and develop an interpretative synthesis, which generates new conceptual understandings [35–37]. In health service research, the meta-ethnographic approach has been widely employed because of its capacity to generate new understandings on how people experience their health and well-being [37].
This study aims to synthesise the findings of discrete qualitative studies reporting the lived experiences, views and preferences of children and young people with DCD using a meta-ethnographic approach. Synthesis of the available qualitative research with children and young people with DCD will inform future research by mapping research conducted to date and has the potential to inform intervention and service delivery through generating new conceptual understandings of the experiences of this group.
Methods
This qualitative evidence synthesis used a meta-ethnographic approach, following the seven-stage process described by [36], and the eMERGe guidance [35]. A detailed study protocol has been published previously [38] and this meta-ethnography is registered on PROSPERO, registration number CRD42019129178.
Search strategy
The search strategy was developed from reviews examining DCD literature [39] and qualitative research [40]. A combination of keywords, thesaurus and MeSH terms were utilised (an example of the search strategy used in MEDLINE is presented in S1 File. Ten databases were searched; Academic Search Complete, AMED, ERIC, CINAHL, MEDLINE, PsychArticles, PsychInfo, EMBASE, SPORTDiscus, and Web of Science between March and April 2019. In the first week of June 2020, the search strategy was administered across the 10 databases in order to identify any further articles that may have been published between April 2019 and June 2020. Searches were limited to English language publications, but no limits were applied to the date of publication. The PRISMA-checklist for systematic reviews was used to illustrate the search strategy procedures[41], which is presented in S2 File.
Inclusion & exclusion criteria
Primary research studies using qualitative methods of data collection and analysis to explore children and young people’s (5–18 years) views, opinions, and experiences of living with DCD, were included. Full details of the inclusion and exclusion criteria are presented in S3 File. Studies were excluded if (a) they included participants with a range of neurodevelopmental diagnoses and the qualitative data for those with DCD could not be extracted, or (b) the data presented was aggregated (for example, a mix of parent and child data that cannot be easily identifiable).
Screening
Once duplicates were removed, the first author (ÁOD) screened papers by title and abstract against the pre-designed inclusion/exclusion criteria (S3 File). KR screened 10% of papers by title and abstract to check for consistency. Papers included for full-text review were read and screened by ÁOD and KR. Each reviewer independently considered the paper’s relevance to this qualitative synthesis, any differences of opinion were resolved via discussion. The entire screening process is presented via a PRISMA flowchart in (Fig 1).
Data extraction and analysis
This study followed the analytic and synthesis phases of meta-ethnography outlined by Noblit and Hare [36], and the eMERGe guidance [35]. The raw data of meta-ethnography comprises participants’ views, explanations or perceptions of a phenomena in original studies, and the study authors’ interpretations and conceptualisations of these data [42]. Participants’ descriptions of their experience of the phenomena, (in this case; living with DCD), are labelled first-order constructs [42]. The researchers’ analysis and interpretation of these first-order constructs are labelled as the second-order constructs [42]. Third-order constructs are the synthesis of the researchers’ interpretations of the second-order constructs [42]. Noblit and Hare detail the importance of constructing ‘adequate interpretative explanations’ through the selection of key metaphors or concepts while preserving the sense of original accounts (pg.13 36). Metaphors are “what others might call themes, perspectives, organizers and /or concepts revealed by qualitative studies” (pg.15 36). The full-text pdfs of included papers were uploaded to QSR International’s Nvivo 12 software, so that first-order and second-order constructs could be extracted and inductively interpreted to identify the key concepts/metaphors. This software platform provided a useful tool to support the documentation of interpretative concepts generated as the data were repeatedly read and interpreted [38].
Simultaneously, two independent reviewers used a data extraction tool designed specifically for this study to extract and collate information on the characteristics of each study, including; citation, study setting/country, sample size, participant characteristics, aims of the study, data collection and methods, and summary of findings. The extracted data were tabulated to support a broad overview of the included studies [43]. During the synthesis process, the researchers returned to reading and re-reading the full text papers to facilitate immersion in the data and to enhance the analytical rigour and synthesis [35, 36, 43]. To support analytic rigour, one conceptually rich study [44] and one study identified to have some methodological limitations [45] were intentionally selected, read, and independently interpreted by two authors (ÁOD & KR). The interpretative concepts generated by both researchers were discussed until agreement was reached. A similar process was applied to the remaining papers by ÁOD, whereby, interpretative concepts were generated from the extracted data. The research team convened several meetings to discuss these third-order concepts and similarities and differences between the concepts, in order to translate the studies into one another [36, 42]. ‘Translation’ of studies in meta-ethnography is described by Noblit and Hare as a process where studies are treated as analogies and compared with one another [36]. This method of constant comparison of the data identified that the key concepts across the studies were similar and could be added together, thus allowing the researchers to reciprocally translate the studies into one another [36, 42]. During meta-ethnography when the relationship between studies is considered researchers, decide if the synthesis is a refutational synthesis (differing accounts) or a reciprocal synthesis (accounts being similar) [42, 43]. Although there was a difference in the age ranges across the included studies, the accounts were not in opposition to each other. The variance in age ranges from childhood to adolescence helped to support the interpretative explanations developed in the synthesis [36]. Through discussions, these third-order constructs were further refined and developed. The final stages phase involved the research team synthesising the third-order constructs into a line-of-argument, which provides greater conceptual understanding to the phenomena of interest as a whole; that is children and young people’s perspectives and experiences of everyday life and living with DCD.
The researchers identify their philosophical position as interpretivist, this position aligns with the aim of the study and the assumptions underpinning meta-ethnography. Noblit and Hare [36] highlighted that a meta-ethnographic synthesis exposes much about the perspective of the synthesizer as it does about the body of the synthesis. For that reason, the first author who is an occupational therapist with an extensive clinical background in working with children and young people with DCD in paediatric services was cognisant of the potential influence of prior experience on her interpretations. However, the researchers (KR, MS & SC) do not share this background and have extensive qualitative research experience. Throughout, the study reflective discussions within the research team challenged the first author’s interpretations and meanings of conceptual concepts. However, ultimately, the authors acknowledge that our interpretations of the key concepts and subsequent reciprocal translations are only one potential reading of the studies [36].
Quality appraisal of the included studies
The methodological congruency of the included papers was appraised using the JBI Checklist [46]. Two reviewers (ÁOD and KR) independently appraised each paper. The JBI checklist is deemed to be one of the most sensitive tools when examining methodological validity, given its focus on congruity [47]. Each item was recorded as “Yes”, “No”, “Unclear” or” Not applicable”. Once complete, the appraisal findings were contrasted, variations in decisions were examined and consensus was reached via discussion between ÁOD and KR. In addition, quality appraisal decisions were discussed amongst the wider research team. Toye and colleagues [48] acknowledge that current quality appraisal checklists can produce inconsistent decisions. The research team discussions focused not just on methods alone but on the contribution of individual papers and the strength of its concepts to facilitate the generation of interpretative concepts and subsequent conceptual insight to the meta-ethnography [48]. For this reason, the researchers prioritised the discussion of decisions to facilitate consensus.
Results
Study selection
Initial searches yielded 8657 results, 6453 after removing duplicates. Screening by title and abstract excluded 6393 results, leaving 60 studies for full text review. Twelve studies met the inclusion criteria at this stage (March 2019). In the first week of June 2020 the search was updated 1614 articles were identified, after removing duplicates 1205 papers were screened. A further three papers were included. Fig 1 presents a PRISMA Flowchart diagram, detailing the entire process, which led to the inclusion of 15 studies.
Study characteristics
The characteristics of the fifteen included papers are presented in Table 1. Six of the studies were conducted in the United Kingdom [31, 45, 49–52], five in Canada [33, 34, 44, 53, 54], one in Austria/ Italy [55], one in Brazil [56], one in Belgium [57], and one in New Zealand [58]. In total, data from one hundred and nine participants were included in the qualitative synthesis. Nine papers had participants with a mean age ranging from 6.9 years to 11 years [33, 34, 45, 49, 54–58]. Five papers had participants with a mean age range of 13 years to 14.9 years [31, 50–53] and one study included young adults aged 19–25 years who reflected upon their experiences as an adolescent [44]. Two papers included data from one sample of young people interviewed over a two-year period [51, 52].
Quality appraisal
Appraisal of the included papers highlighted a large variation in methodological quality across the studies as seen in Table 2. Ten studies presented the philosophical perspective on which the study was based and a methodological approach which was congruent with the perspective [31, 34, 44, 51, 52, 54–58]. However, the philosophical or theoretical perspective was not clearly represented in the remaining five papers [33, 45, 49, 50, 53]. The majority of studies described an appropriate qualitative methodology for addressing the research question or objectives. Two areas rarely addressed were the researcher’s cultural and theoretical orientation, or the influence of the researcher on the research and vice versa. The reporting of methodological quality was deemed unclear across many items in one study [45]. However, we chose to include the paper, as we believed that the representation of participants and their experiences was clear. Across all studies, participants were clearly represented from the data presented in the findings. One study was included even though it did not include a statement about ethical approval; however, the authors did describe the process for gaining child assent and parental consent [55]. No studies were excluded based on quality.
Synthesis
This meta-ethnographic synthesis of first and second-order constructs produced three interrelated themes (third order constructs); a) ‘It’s harder than it should be’: Navigating daily activities b) Fitting in, and c) ‘So what? I drop things’: Strategies and supports to mitigate challenges.
‘It’s harder than it should be’: Navigating daily activities.
This theme relates to the difficulties children and young people with DCD face performing everyday activities. Children and young people describe the influence of personal factors (such as self-perceptions, motivation, and age) and environmental factors (such as family and school context and support) on their capacity to navigate and perform daily life activities.
The experience of struggling to learn and perform everyday activities and routines was frequently reported across the included studies [31, 33, 34, 44, 45, 49, 51, 52, 55, 57]. Typical childhood activities such as learning to ride a bicycle required additional support and extended periods of practice to master skills and build stamina [33, 34, 44, 49, 52]. In late adolescence, different activity challenges arose such as learning to drive [44, 51].
Children and young people evaluated their performance and functional abilities in everyday scenarios against siblings and peer’s performance, and parental expectations [31, 33, 34, 44, 50, 52, 53, 55, 56]. Self-appraisal was often critical when performance did not align with context expectations [31, 34, 44, 50, 52, 53]. Participants used words like “stupid clumsiness and awkward” to describe themselves and their motor difficulties [44]. Challenging performance in everyday activities contributed to a sense of personal inadequacy for some participants [34, 44, 50, 52, 55]. Negative self-perceptions and fear of exposing their performance difficulties influenced participants’ engagement and willingness to try social and physical activities [34, 44, 50, 52]. As articulated by this young boy: ‘‘I always think I’m a loser and um, you know feel kind of sad for quite a long time but I’ll get over it. It’s really sad and you don’t think you can do it. And you stop trying” [34]. These perceptions left some children and young people feeling different from their peers and siblings [31, 34, 44, 52, 53] as illustrated in this young person’s description:
I couldn’t write properly, I couldn’t play sports properly, and I was always spilling things or breaking things. It just makes me feel so—like you’re so different from other people, and nobody can ever really understand. It would make me feel like there was something wrong with me personally, when really I couldn’t really help it [44].
Requiring parental assistance to complete or accomplish activities left some children feeling embarrassed and guilty, and perceiving themselves as a burden on their parents, because they could not perform activities independently or without creating a mess [34, 44, 50, 52]. However, in most cases children and young people recognised their need for parental assistance and perceived assistance positively [31, 33, 34, 44, 50, 52, 57, 58]. Participants spoke frankly about parental support, which was provided in various ways, such as teaching skills, scaffolding the demands of the task, helping the child understand their difficulties [34, 52, 57], advocating for academic support [31, 52], and providing practical and emotional support that facilitated participation in community and leisure activities [50, 57, 58].
My mum and dad on a Sunday go to the gym with me… they encourage me to do more like stuff in the gym… If they didn’t go to the gym with me than I probably wouldn’t do any physical activity at all [50].
Motor performance difficulties impacted on school life [31, 34, 44, 45, 50–53] where some perceived that they were not performing adequately and were at the lower end of achievement for their grade [31, 34, 51, 55]. Specific school-based activities that were challenging included handwriting, self-management of learning, physical activity, sports, and recess time.
Physical education classes, school sports, recess time, and out of school activity were associated with performance difficulties and emotional distress [31, 34, 44, 45, 50, 52, 53]. Factors such as fatigue, pain, poor performance endurance and fear of injury influenced participant’s motivation, involvement, and enjoyment of physical activities in school activities and out-of-school activities [33, 34, 44, 50, 57, 58]. As summed up by this participant “It’s just hard to get going, hard to keep going” [50].
Handwriting was experienced as hard work and a significant problem that affected participation in school [31, 34, 44, 51, 52], which in some cases warranted intervention [31, 33, 34, 49, 51, 52]. Handwriting difficulties were pervasive and included legibility, letter formation, speed of performance, fatigue, and pain [31, 34, 49, 51, 52]. Participants received support via targeted handwriting practice, use of computers and tablets, typing programs and personnel-based support such as scribes and Learning Support Assistants [31, 34, 44, 51, 52]. However, access to learning supports was not equal; in one study participants from lower socio-economic backgrounds were promised additional help and resources but these did not materialise [31]. Young people expressed relief when the environmental context changed and writing was no longer a priority [44]. In contrast, children and young people perceived generic strategies as unhelpful or ineffective when imposed without considering their individual needs [34, 51].
I’ve done three, two, typing programs both, the whole thing at my home and I’ve done the same typing program almost twice in school and it’s still, I’m still looking down and I can’t really type fast [34].
The perceptions of others, including teachers, peers, parents, and siblings influenced participant’s perceptions of their handwriting, and scholastic success [31, 34, 44, 51, 52].
If someone said something to me, like a teacher about handwriting or something. I would just be less confident for the rest of the day [51].
The teachers’ level of understanding of handwriting difficulties varied with some teachers compounding children’s difficulties, as illustrated by the quote.
It’s harder than it should be. And then most of my teachers except for one were stupid and like, didn’t notice… I lost a lot of marks in school because I had messy handwriting which I didn’t think was fair… They said oh, I’m just lazy… So how would they feel if someone called them lazy when they’re working their hardest. Or when you make a mistake and they point it out to you every waking moment [34].
Other participants reflected negatively upon their handwriting and scholastic performance compared to their siblings and peers [31, 34, 52, 56]. As highlighted by this adolescent boy, “My sister’s got better writing than me … Don’t get me wrong, I’m an intelligent person, but like I can’t even write. It’s making me fill up” [52]. In contrast, other participants reported that they were doing well in school and valued the positive reinforcement that they received from teachers, parents and siblings [31, 34, 51]. Extra special attention was not wanted [31]; rather participants appreciated help when teachers and learning support staff demonstrated an understanding and awareness of their needs [31, 34, 51].
Challenges with executive functioning skills and self-management affected participants’ independence in school, during class-based learning experiences, and out of school activities. Children and young people detailed difficulties with the planning, organisation and remembering personal belongings, concentration, and memory skills [31, 34, 51, 52, 55]. Participants were aware of the challenges that they faced understanding and processing new information in-class [31]. As described by this participant; ‘It is not because he or she is going fast it is just me not knowing because I don’t understand what they mean’ [31, 52]. In contrast, some participants’ used supportive strategies, such as sitting beside a friend or a Learning support assistant so that they could explain the information again [57]. While, others felt that an individualised learning approach suited their ability to learn new skills and information [31, 44, 52].
‘Fitting in’.
This theme relates to children and young people with DCD’s desire to participate and to be socially included in peer interactions and experiences in everyday life situations. Friendships were desired, and positively experienced however, experiences of marginalisation; exclusion and bullying because of the mismatch between their skills and performance expectations were common. Across the included studies, friendships were reported as desired [31, 34, 44, 45, 52–55, 57], and provide a forum for fun, acceptance of individual difference, and shared interests [31, 52, 53, 57]. Children with DCD describe pleasure in playing with others, as it facilitates a sense of ‘inclusion’, ‘companionship’ and ‘friendship’ [57]. In the school context, the protective nature of friendships against social exclusion, being teased or bullied was deemed particularly important [31, 52–54]. Friends were viewed, as “one of the best things about school” [31], and the opportunity to hang out with friends outside school was highly regarded [31, 52]. Friendships helped children and young people to evaluate and identify positive self-perceptions [31, 51, 52].
A lot of my mates say I am funny; I think I am quite funny and I am quite strong and um, my mates have told me I am quite reliable [31].
Having additional needs or being perceived as different from their peers was the impetus that led some young people to forming social connections, but such connections also helped participants to reframe their perceptions about difference [31, 52, 53].
They’ve [his group of friends] got like all different like talents, if you get my drift like…but most are into different sports and that [31].
Children transitioning from primary to secondary school worried about making new friends, [45]. Some noted that with transition to high school "that certain people made friends just because they were on a sports team" [44], reflecting the premise that participation in physical activities provided increased social opportunities [44].
Social and attitudinal environments influenced acceptance and the opportunity to participate in activities. Children with DCD commonly experienced challenging social situations, particularly with peers, ranging from marginalisation and exclusion to bullying [31, 34, 44, 45, 50, 52–56]. Exclusion typically occurred in the school context [31, 34, 44, 45, 50, 52, 53, 56]. Children and young people perceived that the most noteworthy barriers that led to exclusion or being left out from participating in specific groups or activities was the ‘mismatch’ between the performance standard and their ‘skills’ and ‘competences’ [34, 44, 50, 52, 53, 57]. As described by this young person, “I tried to help earlier but I wrecked everything, so he won’t let me do anything that involves making characters” [52].
Bullying was encountered or feared in the school setting by many [31, 34, 44, 45, 50, 52, 53]. Participants articulated experiences of negative name-calling, being laughed at, and being teased or ridiculed about their motor performance [31, 34, 44, 45, 50, 52, 53, 56].
Everybody laughs at me when I try to run on the grass… They sometimes bully me and chase me and all that [34].
Being teased or ridiculed also occurred in the home setting between siblings, as described by this participant, her brother had told her that ‘she was going to be a prostitute when she grew older as she did not like school’ [31]. Reported incidents of physical harassment and assault, were traumatic and left the individuals feeling angry, vulnerable, and unsafe [31, 34, 52].
Sometimes I don’t even want to go to class… it was fine when people knew [about the diagnosis] and just said stuff, but now they’re going on like about stabbing people [31].
Participation in school sports activities was often associated with distressing experiences of marginalisation and bullying [34, 44, 50, 52, 53]. Negative self-perceptions result from this mismatch between performance and expectations.
Teacher awareness also influenced the experience of school sport,
I have had some really bad experiences with PE… the teacher can get frustrated quite easily, ‘they were like screaming at me and saying ‘run properly boy’… I just don’t like to be yelled at to do stuff [50].
Teacher awareness and attitude towards bullying was considered important [31, 52], but children and young people did not typically perceive teachers as a helpful resource to dealing with bullying [45, 52]. Marginalisation and social exclusion were associated with feelings of loneliness, sadness, and frustration [34, 53], and a wide range of negative self-perceptions [31, 34, 44, 52, 53]. Young people perceived that they were viewed as different, as reported by this 14-year-old girl, “they just think that we’re weird” [53].
‘So what? I drop things’: Strategies and supports to mitigate challenges.
Although children and young people with DCD describe numerous challenges and difficult experiences in everyday life, embedded within these descriptions were multiple supports and strategies used to mitigate challenges and facilitate participation. Personal factors such as the child and young person with DCD’s preferences towards activities and life situations, their sense of self, age, and insight into their health condition influenced their capacity to join in and be involved.
Across the studies, many participants spoke positively about their everyday lives. They experienced enjoyment in family, play and leisure-based activities, which they chose to, participate in, and performed competently [31, 33, 34, 44, 49, 50, 52, 53, 56–58]. As described by this child’s preferred activities in school and the community context; “Hide and Seek… Chess, we play dolls at school… Soccer, I like dodgeball too”[56]. Participants focused upon their interests, strengths and what they could do, or a unique attribute and personal quality [31, 34, 44, 52].
There is always something that you can do that other kids can’t…. Like your talent, everyone has a talent [34].
Although some participants were unsure of how they would manage in the future, for others, the challenges they faced did not limit their future goals and aspirations such as learning to drive or going to college or university [31, 52].
Another common strategy was adopting a “just do it” attitude [31, 34, 44, 51, 52]. Participants’ descriptions revealed a growth mindset, towards practice and perseverance of skills/activities, that they had “not yet” mastered [31, 34, 44, 52]. With age, teenagers reflected that they had developed the competencies to perform most basic self-care activities, “not like perfect, but to a standard that’s OK” [51]. Participants’ interests and motivation to join a peer play or recreational activity influenced their desire to persist with and practice challenging activities [31, 34, 44, 49, 52, 55]. Equally, as children and young people mature, they recognise and accept the challenges they experience, which in turn helps them to develop autonomy and confidence to manage everyday life scenarios [31, 34, 44, 51, 52].
What I was good at, I kept doing, and what I wasn’t good at, and I stopped doing. Then I started being more confident in everything I did because everything I did I could do [44].
Understanding the diagnosis of DCD influenced participants sense of self that is it helped young people’s self-confidence and self-perceptions [31, 34, 44, 49, 51, 52] and allowed participants to accept and embrace their difference [31, 51].
Knowing that I’ve got it. That I’m not just a bit weird …. I’m not ashamed of it because it kind of makes me who I am. I think that everyone has their problems [31].
Therapeutic intervention helped others to understand their difficulties; in turn, this enhanced their confidence, and perception that they were not the only person to struggle with everyday activities [31, 49]. Role models such as the actor Daniel Radcliffe who identifies as having DCD helped others to associate with a positive identify [51]. The perception that DCD was a hidden disability was present “nobody ever really understand…it would make me feel like there was something wrong with me personally” [44]. However, a more common perspective was that performance and context expectations were a social construct, and therefore participants did not internalise their experience of everyday activities [31, 34, 44, 51, 52].
When I look at it, it’s not a problem… . I never felt like it was me as much as it was something imposed on me by other people because they were the ones who were criticizing or passing comments… . So what? I drop things. It doesn’t matter. It only becomes a problem when you think that dropping things isn’t normal [44].
Children valued the opportunity to set personal goals for skills/activities that they wanted to develop and practice [34, 49, 55]. Goals were realised through therapy for some children [49, 55], while others engaged in extensive practice with a parent [34], where tailored practice enhanced children’s confidence and self-perceptions [34, 49]. The sense of achievement and involvement when children mastered cycling was clear: “I’m better at riding a bike now…we [family]was in Yorkshire…I was OK on this bike ride…it [therapy sessions] just makes me think.. ‘I can do it’ so I won’t have that much problem pedalling [49].
Children practiced adaptive self-exclusion from social everyday activities that were not inclusive, potentially exposed their difficulties, or where they lacked the self-belief that they could participate successfully [34, 44, 50–52, 56–58]. For example, a 13-year old with DCD reported “If it was a team sport with people that I didn’t know particularly well, then I wouldn’t particularly want to get involved in case I got it wrong” [52]. Whilst self-exclusion was a common strategy for many children this strategy was not without its drawbacks. As highlighted by some young peoples’ reflections, who wished that, they had persevered for longer with activities, as, this strategy had limited subsequent participation opportunities as an adult [44].
“Now that I’m older, I kind of wish I had gotten into things earlier, because now I wish I was involved in some sort of group activity, sport, something, you know? My friends now, some of them are cheerleaders, and some of them [play] lacrosse or whatever, and I still don’t have anything” [44].
Many children and young people with DCD described the use of humour or sarcasm as a strategy to support inclusion [34, 44, 53]. As revealed by this 12-year-old boy with DCD: “Mostly, I just go around collecting the dodge balls and say “dodge balls! Need ammo? Here!” [53]. Learning to laugh at themselves and joke about their activity performance were viewed as means to boost involvement in an activity, reduce frustration, and deflect negative peer attention [34, 44, 53].
Line of argument synthesis
The results of the 15 papers were reciprocally translatable and led the development of three interpretative themes. Higher order interpretation of these themes identified that the experiences of children and young people with DCD can be understood as the experience of psycho-emotional disablism, a concept developed within the field of disability studies. Carol Thomas [59] conceptualises psycho-emotional disablism as occurring when individuals with impairments are ‘hurt by the reactions and behaviours of those around them, made to feel worthless, of lesser value, unattractive, hopeless, stressed or insecure’.
Across the included studies, children with DCD described struggling to meet their parents, teachers, peers and broader society expectations around performance of activities. The mismatch between their abilities and performance norms in many cases led to a cascade of negative consequences including self-criticism, negative self-appraisal, bullying, victimisation, marginalisation and exclusion by peers and siblings. Across the included studies numerous examples of psycho-emotional disablism arising from relationships with other people, were reported, for example critical comments such as teasing or actions such as exclusion. Another manifestation of psycho-emotional disablism, internalised oppression, was also evident in self-criticism and negative self-perceptions. Further underscoring the significance of the social and attitudinal environment on the experience of children with DCD, those who encountered positive and encouraging social and attitudinal environments across a range of environmental contexts navigated situations with greater ease and detailed experiences that are more positive. In response to disablism, children and young people developed numerous strategies and drew on available supports to facilitate their involvement in activities and enhance social inclusion across life situations. Children with DCD revealed that they accessed support to diminish the mismatch between their performance and the expectations of others, including seeking and accepting assistance, focusing on their strengths and talents, accepting, and embracing their difference, adopting a “just do it” attitude, setting personal goals, self-exclusion from some social activities, using humour or sarcasm, and viewing performance and context expectations as a social construct.
Discussion
This synthesis reveals important insights into the lived experiences of children and young people with DCD. Three interrelated themes were identified and labelled, as ‘It is harder than it should be’: Navigating daily, Fitting in, ‘So What? I drop things’. Across the studies in this review, participants experienced a wide array of challenges learning and performing new skills and activities, participating in school, and experienced social exclusion from activities such as play and leisure. This aligns with much of the quantitative research to date on DCD in childhood, which has focused upon the predictors of patterns of participation [60]. However, this qualitative review focused upon the lived experience of children with DCD and exposes the array of self-developed strategies and successfully accessed supports used by participants to deal with these challenges and navigate everyday life situations. Findings of this synthesis resonate with the social model of disability and the ICF.
Considering the synthesis findings in the context of the social model of disability and the ICF suggests that the social, attitudinal, and physical environment significantly influences children and young people’s everyday experiences. The synthesis revealed that children with DCD experience bullying and various types of victimisation [61], including, verbal (name-calling, teasing), social (social exclusion, marginalisation), and physical (physical assault) in the school setting. Children with disabilities are at increased risk of bullying in schools in comparison to their typically developing peers [62, 63]. The high prevalence of bullying experienced by children with disabilities is very concerning as strong evidence supports that bullying victimisation in childhood is associated with a wide range of adverse mental health and psychosocial outcomes [64]. Bullying of children because they live with socially devalued identities or characteristics, such as disability, is described as stigma-based bullying and requires bullying interventions specifically focusing on stigma-related factors [65]. Within school environments, greater support to help teachers and peers reconceptualise disability as a social issue may help to challenge notions of marginalisation and discrimination based on socially valued characteristics such as motor proficiency.
The World Health Organisation [66] and childhood disability researchers [67] recognise the importance of contextual factors such as the environment on the health and well-being of individuals. Research has shown that environmental factors (physical, social, and attitudinal) mediate the relationship between child factors and participation outcomes for children and youth with disabilities [68, 69]. For all children, the family represents the most crucial environment, and parents are considered a critical contextual factor in a child’s life [66, 67]. The synthesis findings highlighted the profound influence of the social and attitudinal environmental on children’s experiences within the family context. In line with recent findings regarding the association between family factors and participation of children with a disability [70]; this review found that where parents and siblings understood the child’s strengths and capabilities, participants navigated daily life with greater ease, experienced greater involvement in activities within the home and community environment, and described positive self-perceptions. Conversely, where the home environment was not supportive of the child’s performance and involvement, negative emotions, conflict with parents and siblings, and negative self-perceptions were typical. Our findings support the importance of health service interventions targeted at the family environment. Family-centred approaches that enhance the family’s capacity to understand their child and build competency to resolve challenges may be most beneficial to the child and family [67].
In addition, the school environment was a contextual factor, which significantly influenced opportunities for social inclusion and involvement in activities. Our findings demonstrate the significant influence of teachers and peers on participation and the subsequent positive or negative self-perceptions and school-based experiences of children and young people with DCD. Evidence suggests that teacher awareness and understanding of DCD is poor [71]. The findings of this synthesis highlighted that school sports and physical education were perceived as particularly challenging experiences, which led to incidents of ridicule, self-exclusion, marginalisation, and reluctance to participate due to poor teacher understanding and awareness. As noted by [72], teachers’ knowledge, understanding and confidence about teaching students with a disability is crucial to facilitating inclusive and effective Physical Education (PE). In line with previous research, our results suggest that children and young people’s functional abilities with handwriting, motor, organisation and planning skills influenced their capacity to complete specific activities in school [73]. However, similar to recent findings, involvement in school-based activities was also heavily influenced by the interaction between personal factors such as motivation, interest, preference and environment factors such as the adults, peers, the school structures and routines as well as access to assistive technology and objects [73]. These findings highlight the need for interventions that target teacher collaboration between health professional and educator, in order to build teachers’ knowledge, awareness and capacity to support children and youths with DCD in school. As seen in Canada, Partnering for Change presents an innovate intervention that promotes a model of health and education partnership, which shifts the focus from individualised support to targeting whole school environmental interventions [74]. Society and systems influence the opportunities for inclusion in many aspects of social life such as education, health and community choices [75]. Therefore, further development and evaluation of targeted social/environmental interventions is warranted as they may provide a means to enhance inclusion and reduce social, physical, and attitudinal barriers faced by children and young people with DCD.
Rosenbaum and Gorter [67] provide a novel representation of the ICF concepts, which they define as the F-words (fitness, function, friends, family, fun and future). The results of this study converge with Rosenbaum and Gorter’s [67] representation of the ICF concepts highlighting the importance of friendships, peer social acceptance and fun to support participation in everyday situations. Children and young people with DCD valued friendships and the opportunity for inclusion in social and physical activities with peers across environments. These findings align with previous evidence, which suggest children with disabilities perceive activity experiences as positive, when completed with peers and friends and they are fun [57, 76, 77]. Unsurprisingly, friendships for children with disabilities are associated with greater psychosocial wellbeing [78], social inclusion [79], and healthier identify formation in adolescence [80]. Previous research has revealed that children with a disability face greater risk of social exclusion [81]. However, social inclusion is possible when personal and social resources for the children with a disability are present, including, the ability to make friends, participate in community activities, engage in leisure and play, and have access to quality inclusive practices in the classroom [81]. Our findings emphasise the importance of attending to the social environment so that children and young people with DCD have the opportunity to be involved in meaningful activities that promote social interaction and friendship over time.
In our review, some participants drew on personal resources or accessed supports that facilitated inclusion. Personal resources included a “just do it” attitude, recognising their strengths, accepting and embracing their difference, using humour or sarcasm, and viewing performance and context expectations as a social construct. Our findings corroborate the significance of personal resources such as preferences, interests, and motivations to influence and sustain participation in leisure and school-based activities [73, 76, 82], while supports accessed included, friendships, and support from family and teachers. Positive relationships are considered an important protective factor to help children overcome the risk of social exclusion [81, 83]. The findings of review highlight how the interaction between the personal and environmental factors shaped the lived experiences of children and young people with DCD.
Our integration of findings clearly show that some children and young people experience difficulties because of the mismatch between performance expectations, and their relationships with others including family, teachers and friends. We developed a line of argument and present the experience of children and young people with DCD as reflective of adopting the social relational perspective of the social model of disability [59, 84]. This theoretical perspective provides a lens to contextualise how social and attitudinal restrictions of activity influence the experiences of children and young people with DCD. Children and young people’s perspectives reflect ‘psycho-emotional disablism’ [59]. The social behaviours of others, such as family members, teachers and peers had the potential to undermine their psycho-emotional wellbeing and impose social restrictions of activity. Accordingly, changes are required within the social structures, which create prejudice towards individuals with impairment not to the individual’s psychological adjustment [59], hence in keeping with the social model of disability, interventions that target sigma may be helpful approaches [65].
The review findings corroborate the need for greater attention to the child’s perspective in research and practice, recognising that there are potential discrepancies between children and parents’ perspectives. A recent survey of perceived support for children with DCD in Canadian schools surveyed parents of children with DCD rather than children themselves [85]. Izadi-Najafabadi and colleagues found that the attitudes and actions of teachers, staff, school-related policies and procedures were perceived as adequate by parents [85], whereas the findings of this review reflects that children and young people do not always perceives attitudes and actions of school staff as supportive. Accessing the lived experience of school for children with DCD is important, as children can articulate their experiences and richly inform others on the issues affecting their lives. Another important distinction between the perspective of children and parents is that young people with coordination difficulties, more so than their parents, prioritise friendship as means to promoting self-esteem [86]. The findings of this review corroborate this perspective, friendships were highly regarded by children and young people and influenced their self-perceptions. This review underscores the importance of health professionals exploring the child and young person’s perspectives and priorities for treatment, in conjunction with the parental perspectives in order to build family capacity to manage everyday life situations, line with best practice recommendations [6, 87]. The synthesis findings emphasise the need for professionals to design interventions, which target priority outcomes for friendship, social inclusion and greater understanding and awareness in the school setting, as these are important to children and young people with DCD. Professionals should also explore the child’s strength, and strategies used to navigate everyday life prior to recommending or commencing intervention.
Strengths and limitations
A major strength of this paper is that it is the first meta-ethnography to focus on the experience of children and young people with DCD. This synthesis addresses the unequal attention conferred to parents versus children’s perspectives. Research examining children’s perspective is essential if we are to improve the lives of children [88].
A strength of this paper is the robust conduct of the review; the authors adhered to the eMERGe and PRIMSA guidelines. However, a limitation of this review was the quality of included studies varied. Furthermore, the phenomenon of interest examined across the included studies was diverse, which resulted in a focus on certain aspects or dimensions of children’s and young people’s experience in some papers. For example, Hessell and colleagues’ examination of the experience of a gymnastics club for children with DCD [58], or Armitage and colleagues’ investigation of children’s perceptions and experiences of occupational therapy intervention [49].
Implications for practice
The findings of the synthesis point to the need for health professionals working with this population to consider and address the social and attitudinal environments in which children and young people participate and live, but especially the school environment. It is important that interventions, which support teacher knowledge, awareness and understanding of DCD, be developed. Such interventions are one element of promoting a positive social and attitudinal school environment for children with DCD. For example, increasing teacher awareness and recognising the challenges associated with fatigue, fear of injury or poor performance in front of peers during physical education classes may help to support involvement.
Health service provision should adopt a family-centred approach that enhances the capacity of the family to support engagement and performance in activities and everyday situations. Augmenting parents understanding of DCD may be helpful in addressing potential negative child self-perceptions arising from parental expectations. Finally, children and young people described numerous successful strategies to deal with challenging experiences, victimisation, and exclusion. Thus, greater attention is required by professionals to identify children’s capabilities and strategies, which may be leveraged to address challenging social interactions and navigate daily life activities.
Future research
Most qualitative studies included in this review explored lived experiences of children with DCD in general. We identified no qualitative studies focusing exclusively on the experiences of friendship or romantic relationships, education, or transition to work or vocational roles or parenting with DCD for example, and these topics could be the focus of future work. Future research needs to explore children’s experiences of friendships and social inclusion. Furthermore, research needs to broaden the focus of examination; and explore how children and young people with DCD deal with life situations and specific activities. This review highlighted that children and young people implement many strategies to support their involvement in everyday life. Children’s experiences and coping skills needs to be examined in-depth to inform future intervention development and help to guide recommendations for practice. Moreover, future studies should be designed with Public and Patient Involvement from the outset to ensure that the concerns and priorities of children and young people with DCD are addressed in research conducted. Guidelines on co-producing research with young people exist and can provide a framework to support the inclusion of children and young people’s voices throughout the research process [89, 90].
Conclusion
This paper exemplifies the value of qualitative research to inform research, policy, and practice. Our synthesis of fifteen papers produced three interrelated themes; a) ‘It’s harder than it should be’: Navigating daily activities, b) Fitting in, and c) ‘So what? I drop things’: Strategies and supports to mitigate challenges. Children with DCD describe a mismatch between their abilities and performance norms for daily activities that could led to a cascade of negative consequences including negative self-appraisal, bullying and exclusion. In the face of these difficulties, children with DCD described friendships as a forum for fun, acceptance and protective against exclusion and bullying and they described a range of creative and successful strategies they enacted and supports they accessed.
References
- 1.
American Psychiatric Association. Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders: DSM-5. 5th ed. Arlington, VA, USA.: American Psychiatric Publishing Inc; 2013.
- 2. Cairney J, Hay J, Veldhuizen S, Missiuna C, Mahlberg N, Faught BE. Trajectories of relative weight and waist circumference among children with and without developmental coordination disorder. Canadian Medical Association Journal. 2010;182(11):1167–72. pmid:20584932
- 3. Harrowell I, Hollén L, Lingam R, Emond A. Mental health outcomes of developmental coordination disorder in late adolescence. Developmental Medicine & Child Neurology. 2017;59(9):973–9. pmid:28512766
- 4. Omer S, Jijon AM, Leonard HC. Research review: Internalising symptoms in developmental coordination disorder: A systematic review and meta‐analysis. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry. 2018. pmid:30485419
- 5. Harrowell I, Holien L, Lingam R, Emond A. The impact of developmental coordination disorder on educational achievement in secondary school. Research in Developmental Disabilities. 2018;72:13–22. pmid:29080482
- 6. Blank R, Barnett AL, Cairney J, Green D, Kirby A, Polatajko H, et al. International clinical practice recommendations on the definition, diagnosis, assessment, intervention, and psychosocial aspects of developmental coordination disorder. Developmental Medicine & Child Neurology. 2019;61(3):242–85.
- 7. Miyahara M, Hillier SL, Pridham L, Nakagawa S. Task-oriented interventions for children with developmental co-ordination disorder. Cochrane Database of Systematic Reviews. 2017(7). pmid:28758189
- 8. O’Dea Á, Robinson K, Coote S. Effectiveness of interventions to improve participation outcomes for children with developmental coordination disorder: A systematic review. British Journal of Occupational Therapy. 2019;83(4):256–73.
- 9. O’Cathain A, Croot L, Duncan E, Rousseau N, Sworn K, Turner KM, et al. Guidance on how to develop complex interventions to improve health and healthcare. BMJ Open. 2019;9(8):e029954. pmid:31420394
- 10.
Nations U. Convention on the Rights of Persons with Disabilities (CRPD) New York: United Nations; 2006 [cited 2020 18th October]. Available from: https://www.un.org/development/desa/disabilities/convention-on-the-rights-of-persons-with-disabilities.html.
- 11.
Darbyshire P. From research on children to research with children [Guest editorial] 2000.
- 12. Darbyshire P, MacDougall C, Schiller W. Multiple methods in qualitative research with children: more insight or just more? Qualitative research. 2005;5(4):417–36.
- 13. Fattore T, Mason J, Watson E. Locating the child centrally as subject in research: Towards a child interpretation of well-being. Child Indicators Research. 2012;5(3):423–35.
- 14.
Soffer M, Ben-Arieh A. School-aged children as sources of information about their lives. In: Ben-Arieh A, Cashmore J, Goodman G, Melton G, editors. Handbook of child research. London, UK: Sage Publications; 2014. p. 555–74.
- 15.
Swauger M, Castro Ingrid E, Harger B. The Continued Importance of Research with Children and Youth: The “New” Sociology of Childhood 40 Years Later. Researching Children and Youth: Methodological Issues, Strategies, and Innovations. Sociological Studies of Children and Youth. 22: Emerald Publishing Limited; 2017. p. 1–7.
- 16. Alonso Soriano C, Hill EL, Crane L. Surveying parental experiences of receiving a diagnosis of developmental coordination disorder (DCD). Research in Developmental Disabilities. 2015;43:11–20. pmid:26151439
- 17. Mandich AD, Polatajko HJ, Rodger S. Rites of passage: Understanding participation of children with developmental coordination disorder. Human Movement Science. 2003;22(4/5):583. pmid:14624835
- 18. Missiuna C, Moll S, Law M, King S, King G. Mysteries and mazes: Parents’ experiences of children with developmental coordination disorder. Canadian Journal of Occupational Therapy / Revue Canadienne D’Ergothérapie. 2006;73(1):7–17. pmid:16570837
- 19. Summers J, Larkin D, Dewey D. What Impact does Developmental Coordination Disorder have on Daily Routines? International Journal of Disability, Development & Education. 2008;55(2):131–41.
- 20. Stephenson EA, Chesson RA. ’Always the guiding hand’: Parents’ accounts of the long-term implications of developmental co-ordination disorder for their children and families. Child: Care, Health and Development. 2008;34(3):335–43. pmid:18410640
- 21. Bart O, Jarus T, Erez Y, Rosenberg L. How do young children with DCD participate and enjoy daily activities? Res Dev Disabil. 2011;32(4):1317–22. pmid:21397458
- 22. Van der Linde BW, van Netten JJ, Otten B, Postema K, Geuze RH, Schoemaker MM. Activities of Daily Living in Children With Developmental Coordination Disorder: Performance, Learning, and Participation. Physical Therapy. 2015;95(11):1496–506. pmid:26045605
- 23. Jarus T, Lourie-Gelberg Y, Engel-Yeger B, Bart O. Participation patterns of school-aged children with and without DCD. Research in Developmental Disabilities. 2011;32(4):1323–31. pmid:21324639
- 24. Poulsen AA, Johnson H, Ziviani JM. Participation, self-concept and motor performance of boys with developmental coordination disorder: A classification and regression tree analysis approach. Australian Occupational Therapy Journal. 2011;58(2):95–102. pmid:21418232
- 25. Sylvestre A, Nadeau L, Charron L, Larose N, Lepage C. Social participation by children with developmental coordination disorder compared to their peers. Disability and Rehabilitation: An International, Multidisciplinary Journal. 2013;35(21):1814–20.
- 26. Morgan R, Long T. The Effectiveness of Occupational Therapy for Children with Developmental Coordination Disorder: A Review of the Qualitative Literature. British Journal of Occupational Therapy. 2012;75(1):10–8.
- 27. Vroland-Nordstrand K, Eliasson A-C, Jacobsson H, Johansson U, Krumlinde-Sundholm L. Can children identify and achieve goals for intervention? A randomized trial comparing two goal-setting approaches. Developmental Medicine & Child Neurology. 2016;58(6):589–96. pmid:26374194
- 28. Gallagher AL, Murphy CA, Conway PF, Perry A. Engaging multiple stakeholders to improve speech and language therapy services in schools: an appreciative inquiry-based study. BMC Health Services Research. 2019;19(1):226. pmid:30987610
- 29.
Taylor B, Francis K. Qualitative Research in the Health Sciences. London and New Tork: Routledge; 2013.
- 30. Soderback M, Coyne I, Harder M. The importance of including both a child perspective and the child’s perspective within health care settings to provide truly child-centred care. J Child Health Care. 2011;15(2):99–106. pmid:21685225
- 31. Lingam RP, Novak C, Emond A, Coad JE. The importance of identity and empowerment to teenagers with developmental co‐ordination disorder. Child: Care, Health and Development. 2014;40(3):309–18.
- 32. Dunford C, Missiuna C, Street E, Sibert J. Children’s Perceptions of the Impact of Developmental Coordination Disorder on Activities of Daily Living. The British Journal of Occupational Therapy. 2005;68(5):207–14.
- 33. Jasmin E, Tetreault S, Lariviere N, Joly J. Participation and needs of children with developmental coordination disorder at home and in the community: Perceptions of children and parents. Res Dev Disabil. 2018;73:1–13. pmid:29245043
- 34. Zwicker JG, Suto M, Harris SR, Vlasakova N, Missiuna C. Developmental coordination disorder is more than a motor problem: Children describe the impact of daily struggles on their quality of life. British Journal of Occupational Therapy. 2018;81(2):65–73.
- 35. France EF, Cunningham M, Ring N, et al. Improving reporting of meta-ethnography: The eMERGe reporting guidance. BMC Medical Research Methodology. 2019;19(1):25. pmid:30709371
- 36.
Noblit GW, Hare RD. Meta-ethnography: Synthesizing qualitative studies. California: Sage Publications; 1988.
- 37. Ring N, Jepson R, Ritchie K. Methods of synthesizing qualitative research studies for health technology assessment. Int J Technol Assess Health Care. 2011;27(4):384–90. pmid:22004781
- 38. O’Dea Á, Coote S, Robinson K. Children and young people’s experiences of living with developmental coordination disorder/dyspraxia: study protocol for a qualitative evidence synthesis [version 1; peer review: 2 approved]. HRB Open Res. 2019;2(28). pmid:32104780
- 39. Smits-Engelsman B, Vinçon S, Blank R, Quadrado VH, Polatajko H, Wilson PH. Evaluating the evidence for motor-based interventions in developmental coordination disorder: A systematic review and meta-analysis. Research in Developmental Disabilities. 2018;74:72–102. pmid:29413431
- 40. Booth A. Searching for qualitative research for inclusion in systematic reviews: a structured methodological review. Systematic reviews. 2016;5(1):74. pmid:27145932
- 41. Moher D, Liberati A, Tetzlaff J, Altman DG, Group P. Preferred reporting items for systematic reviews and meta-analyses: the PRISMA statement. PLoS med. 2009;6(7):e1000097. pmid:19621072
- 42. Toye F, Seers K, Allcock N, Briggs M, Carr E, Barker K. Meta-ethnography 25 years on: challenges and insights for synthesising a large number of qualitative studies. BMC Medical Research Methodology. 2014;14(1):80. pmid:24951054
- 43. Cahill M, Robinson K, Pettigrew J, Galvin R, Stanley M. Qualitative synthesis: a guide to conducting a meta-ethnography. British Journal of Occupational Therapy. 2018;81(3):129–37.
- 44. Missiuna C, Moll S, King G, Stewart D, Macdonald K. Life experiences of young adults who have coordination difficulties. Canadian Journal of Occupational Therapy / Revue Canadienne D’Ergothérapie. 2008;75(3):157–66. pmid:18615927
- 45. Foulder-Hughes L, Prior C. Supporting pupils with DCD and ASD with the transition to secondary school. Research in Education. 2014;92(1):79–92.
- 46.
Joanna Briggs Institute. JBI critical appraisal checklist for qualitative research. http://joannabriggsorg/assets/docs/critical-appraisal-tools/JBI_Critical_Appraisal-Checklist_for_Qualitative_Research2017pdf. 2017.
- 47. Hannes K, Lockwood C, Pearson A. A Comparative Analysis of Three Online Appraisal Instruments’ Ability to Assess Validity in Qualitative Research. Qualitative Health Research. 2010;20(12):1736–43. pmid:20671302
- 48. Toye F, Seers K, Allcock N, Briggs M, Carr E, Andrews J, et al. ‘Trying to pin down jelly’—exploring intuitive processes in quality assessment for meta-ethnography. BMC Medical Research Methodology. 2013;13(1):46. pmid:23517438
- 49. Armitage S, Swallow V, Kolehmainen N. Ingredients and change processes in occupational therapy for children: a grounded theory study. Scandinavian Journal of Occupational Therapy. 2017;24(3):208–13. pmid:27454899
- 50. Barnett AL, Dawes H, Wilmut K. Constraints and facilitators to participation in physical activity in teenagers with Developmental Co-ordination Disorder: an exploratory interview study. Child Care Health and Development. 2013;39(3):393–403. pmid:22515369
- 51. Payne S, Ward G. Conceptual framework of developmental coordination disorder in adolescence: Findings from a qualitative study. British Journal of Occupational Therapy. 2020;83(4):246–55.
- 52. Payne S, Ward G, Turner A, Clare Taylor M, Bark C. The social impact of living with developmental coordination disorder as a 13-year-old. British Journal of Occupational Therapy. 2013;76(8):362–9.
- 53. Dewey D, Volkovinskaia A. Health-related quality of life and peer relationships in adolescents with developmental coordination disorder and attention-deficit-hyperactivity disorder. Developmental Medicine & Child Neurology. 2018;60(7):711–7. pmid:29611868
- 54. Spencer-Cavaliere N, Watkinson EJ. Inclusion Understood From the Perspectives of Children With Disability. Adapted Physical Activity Quarterly. 2010;27(4):275–93. pmid:20956835
- 55. Costa UM, Brauchle G, Kennedy-Behr A. Collaborative goal setting with and for children as part of therapeutic intervention. Disability and Rehabilitation. 2017;39(16):1589–600. pmid:27385635
- 56. de Medeiros CCM, Buffone F, Schochat E, Araujo CRS. Transcending the problem: perceptions of mothers and children about the impact of developmental coordination disorder in everyday life. Cadernos Brasileiros De Terapia Ocupacional-Brazilian Journal of Occupational Therapy. 2019;27(4):792–805.
- 57. Coussens M, Destoop B, De Baets S, Desoete A, Oostra A, Vanderstraeten G, et al. A Qualitative Photo Elicitation Research Study to elicit the perception of young children with Developmental Disabilities such as ADHD and/or DCD and/or ASD on their participation. PloS one. 2020;15(3):e0229538–e. pmid:32187183
- 58. Hessell S, Hocking C, Davies SG. Participation of boys with Developmental Coordination Disorder in gymnastics. New Zealand Journal of Occupational Therapy. 2010;57(1):14–21.
- 59. Thomas C. Rescuing a social relational understanding of disability. Scandinavian Journal of Disability Research. 2004;6(1):22–36.
- 60. Cairney J, Hay JA, Veldhuizen S, Missiuna C, Faught BE. Developmental coordination disorder, sex, and activity deficit over time: a longitudinal analysis of participation trajectories in children with and without coordination difficulties. Developmental Medicine and Child Neurology. 2010;52(3):E67–E72. pmid:20015253
- 61. Vaillancourt T, Trinh V, McDougall P, Duku E, Cunningham L, Cunningham C, et al. Optimizing Population Screening of Bullying in School-Aged Children. Journal of School Violence. 2010;9(3):233–50.
- 62. Chatzitheochari S, Parsons S, Platt L. Doubly Disadvantaged? Bullying Experiences among Disabled Children and Young People in England. Sociology. 2016;50(4):695–713. pmid:27546915
- 63. Gallagher AL, Galvin R, Robinson K, Murphy C-A, Conway PF, Perry A. The characteristics, life circumstances and self-concept of 13 year olds with and without disabilities in Ireland: A secondary analysis of the Growing Up in Ireland (GUI) study. PLOS ONE. 2020;15(3):e0229599. pmid:32168358
- 64. Moore SE, Norman RE, Suetani S, Thomas HJ, Sly PD, Scott JG. Consequences of bullying victimization in childhood and adolescence: A systematic review and meta-analysis. World journal of psychiatry. 2017;7(1):60–76. pmid:28401049
- 65. Earnshaw VA, Reisner SL, Menino DD, Poteat VP, Bogart LM, Barnes TN, et al. Stigma-based bullying interventions: A systematic review. Developmental Review. 2018;48:178–200. pmid:30220766
- 66.
WHO. International classification of functioning, disability and health: ICF. Geneva: World Health Organisation; 2001.
- 67. Rosenbaum P, Gorter JW. The ‘F-words’ in childhood disability: I swear this is how we should think! Child: Care, Health and Development. 2012;38(4):457–63. pmid:22040377
- 68. Anaby D, Law M, Coster W, Bedell G, Khetani M, Avery L, et al. The mediating role of the environment in explaining participation of children and youth with and without disabilities across home, school, and community. Archives of physical medicine and rehabilitation. 2014;95(5):908–17. pmid:24468018
- 69. Krieger B, Piškur B, Schulze C, Beurskens A, Moser A. Environmental pre-requisites and social interchange: the participation experience of adolescents with autism spectrum disorder in Zurich. Disability and Rehabilitation. 2020:1–14. pmid:32356476
- 70. Arakelyan S, Maciver D, Rush R, O’hare A, Forsyth K. Family factors associated with participation of children with disabilities: a systematic review. Developmental Medicine & Child Neurology. 2019;61(5):514–22. pmid:30613957
- 71. Wilson BN, Neil K, Kamps PH, Babcock S. Awareness and knowledge of developmental co-ordination disorder among physicians, teachers and parents. Child: Care, Health and Development. 2013;39(2):296–300.
- 72. Pocock T, Miyahara M. Inclusion of students with disability in physical education: a qualitative meta-analysis. International Journal of Inclusive Education. 2018;22(7):751–66.
- 73. Maciver D, Rutherford M, Arakelyan S, Kramer JM, Richmond J, Todorova L, et al. Participation of children with disabilities in school: A realist systematic review of psychosocial and environmental factors. PloS one. 2019;14(1). pmid:30695082
- 74. Campbell WN, Missiuna CA, Rivard LM, Pollock NA. "Support for everyone": Experiences of occupational therapists delivering a new model of school-based service. Canadian Journal Of Occupational Therapy Revue Canadienne D’ergotherapie. 2012;79(1):51–9. pmid:22439292
- 75.
Whalley Hammell K. Theoretical Models of Disability. Perspectives on Disability and Rehabilitation: Contesting Assumptions; Challenging Practice: Churchill Livingstone; 2006.
- 76. Powrie B, Kolehmainen N, Turpin M, Ziviani J, Copley J. The meaning of leisure for children and young people with physical disabilities: a systematic evidence synthesis. Developmental Medicine & Child Neurology. 2015;57(11):993–1010. pmid:25939955
- 77. Willis C, Girdler S, Thompson M, Rosenberg M, Reid S, Elliott C. Elements contributing to meaningful participation for children and youth with disabilities: a scoping review. Disability and Rehabilitation. 2017;39(17):1771–84. pmid:27442686
- 78. Whitehouse AJO, Durkin K, Jaquet E, Ziatas K. Friendship, loneliness and depression in adolescents with Asperger’s Syndrome. Journal of Adolescence. 2009;32(2):309–22. pmid:18692233
- 79. Scott HM, Havercamp SM. Promoting wellness in people with intellectual and developmental disabilities through relationships. Current Developmental Disorders Reports. 2018;5(3):150–6.
- 80. Timler A, McIntyre F, Rose E, Hands B. Exploring the influence of self-perceptions on the relationship between motor competence and identity in adolescents. PloS one. 2019;14(11):e0224653–e. pmid:31682621
- 81. Koller D, Pouesard ML, Rummens JA. Defining Social Inclusion for Children with Disabilities: A Critical Literature Review. Children & Society. 2018;32(1):1–13.
- 82. Imms C, Granlund M, Wilson PH, Steenbergen B, Rosenbaum PL, Gordon AM. Participation, both a means and an end: a conceptual analysis of processes and outcomes in childhood disability. Developmental Medicine & Child Neurology. 2017;59(1):16–25.
- 83. Zolkoski SM, Bullock LM. Resilience in children and youth: A review. Children and Youth Services Review. 2012;34(12):2295–303.
- 84. Thomas C. How is disability understood? An examination of sociological approaches. Disability & Society. 2004;19(6):569–83.
- 85. Izadi-Najafabadi S, Ryan N, Ghafooripoor G, Gill K, Zwicker JG. Participation of children with developmental coordination disorder. Research in developmental disabilities. 2019;84:75–84. pmid:30041931
- 86. Eggleston M, Watkins W, Frampton C, Hanger N. Coordination difficulties and self-esteem: The views of children, adolescents, and their parents. Aust Occup Ther J. 2020.
- 87. Forsyth K, Maciver D, Howden S, Owen C, Shepherd C. Developmental Coordination Disorder: A synthesis of evidence to underpin an allied health professions’ framework. International Journal of Disability, Development and Education. 2008;55(2):153–72.
- 88. Moore A, Lynch H. Understanding a child’s conceptualisation of well-being through an exploration of happiness: The centrality of play, people and place. Journal of Occupational Science. 2018;25(1):124–41.
- 89. Hawkins J, Madden K, Fletcher A, Midgley L, Grant A, Cox G, et al. Development of a framework for the co-production and prototyping of public health interventions. BMC Public Health. 2017;17(1):689. pmid:28870192
- 90. Pavarini G, Lorimer J, Manzini A, Goundrey-Smith E, Singh I. Co-producing research with youth: The NeurOx young people’s advisory group model. Health Expectations. 2019;22(4):743–51. pmid:31095837