Skip to main content
Advertisement
Browse Subject Areas
?

Click through the PLOS taxonomy to find articles in your field.

For more information about PLOS Subject Areas, click here.

  • Loading metrics

Basking shark sub-surface behaviour revealed by animal-towed cameras

  • Jessica L. Rudd,

    Roles Data curation, Formal analysis, Investigation, Visualization, Writing – original draft, Writing – review & editing

    Affiliation Hatherly Laboratories, University of Exeter, College of Life & Environmental Sciences, Exeter, United Kingdom

  • Tiago Bartolomeu,

    Roles Methodology, Resources, Software

    Affiliation CEiiA, Matosinhos, Portugal

  • Haley R. Dolton,

    Roles Formal analysis, Investigation, Writing – review & editing

    Affiliation Environment and Sustainability Institute, University of Exeter, Penryn, United Kingdom

  • Owen M. Exeter,

    Roles Methodology, Writing – review & editing

    Affiliation Environment and Sustainability Institute, University of Exeter, Penryn, United Kingdom

  • Christopher Kerry,

    Roles Data curation, Writing – review & editing

    Affiliation Environment and Sustainability Institute, University of Exeter, Penryn, United Kingdom

  • Lucy A. Hawkes ,

    Roles Conceptualization, Data curation, Funding acquisition, Investigation, Methodology, Project administration, Resources, Supervision, Writing – review & editing

    l.hawkes@exeter.ac.uk

    Affiliation Hatherly Laboratories, University of Exeter, College of Life & Environmental Sciences, Exeter, United Kingdom

  • Suzanne M. Henderson,

    Roles Conceptualization, Data curation, Funding acquisition, Investigation, Methodology, Project administration, Writing – review & editing

    Affiliation NatureScot, Inverness, United Kingdom

  • Marcus Shirley,

    Roles Conceptualization, Methodology, Resources, Software

    Affiliation MrROV, Wadebride, Cornwall, United Kingdom

  • Matthew J. Witt

    Roles Conceptualization, Data curation, Funding acquisition, Investigation, Methodology, Project administration, Resources, Supervision, Validation, Writing – review & editing

    Affiliations Hatherly Laboratories, University of Exeter, College of Life & Environmental Sciences, Exeter, United Kingdom, Environment and Sustainability Institute, University of Exeter, Penryn, United Kingdom

Abstract

While biologging tags have answered a wealth of ecological questions, the drivers and consequences of movement and activity often remain difficult to ascertain, particularly marine vertebrates which are difficult to observe directly. Basking sharks, the second largest shark species in the world, aggregate in the summer in key foraging sites but despite advances in biologging technologies, little is known about their breeding ecology and sub-surface behaviour. Advances in camera technologies holds potential for filling in these knowledge gaps by providing environmental context and validating behaviours recorded with conventional telemetry. Six basking sharks were tagged at their feeding site in the Sea of Hebrides, Scotland, with towed cameras combined with time-depth recorders and satellite telemetry. Cameras recorded a cumulative 123 hours of video data over an average 64-hour deployment and confirmed the position of the sharks within the water column. Feeding events only occurred within a metre depth and made up ¾ of the time spent swimming near the surface. Sharks maintained similar tail beat frequencies regardless of whether feeding, swimming near the surface or the seabed, where they spent surprisingly up to 88% of daylight hours. This study reported the first complete breaching event and the first sub-surface putative courtship display, with nose-to-tail chasing, parallel swimming as well as the first observation of grouping behaviour near the seabed. Social groups of sharks are thought to be very short term and sporadic, and may play a role in finding breeding partners, particularly in solitary sharks which may use aggregations as an opportunity to breed. In situ observation of basking sharks at their seasonal aggregation site through animal borne cameras revealed unprecedented insight into the social and environmental context of basking shark behaviour which were previously limited to surface observations.

Introduction

The development of tracking technologies over the past 50 years has revolutionised the field of ecology by collecting minimally biased, continuous data on free-ranging animal movement, behaviour, physiology or environment [16]. While biologging tags have answered a wealth of ecological questions, the drivers and consequences of movement and activity often remain difficult to ascertain, particularly in the marine environment where mobile species are challenging to observe directly. A similar looking dive profile may reflect several types of behaviour [7], while spatial overlap between animals does not necessarily mean that they interact, particularly when data is recorded at different spatial or temporal scales (i.e. fishing vessel tracking data is recorded at coarser resolution than animal-borne devices [8]). While cameras have been deployed on animals since the early 1900s [9], despite device miniaturisation and advances in unit recovery systems (i.e. radio and tracking technologies), applications in behavioural research remain relatively recent.

The use of animal-borne cameras in tandem with environmental or movement sensors has not only been paramount in validating behaviours recorded by tags on free-ranging animals [1012], but has also given new understanding of fine-scale habitat selection, which would remain undetected using conventional telemetry systems. For example camera tags have revealed when dive depth may be constrained by bathymetry and not by behaviour [13], recorded previously unknown interactions between white sharks (Carcharodon carcharias) in kelp forests [14], and sixgill sharks (Hexanchus griseus) associating with rocky outcrops at 700 m depth [11]. Through direct observations of feeding events, animal-borne cameras have also revealed the diet [1519], feeding rates [20, 21], sensory systems [22, 23] and foraging strategies [7, 12, 24] of a range of marine species. For example, little penguins (Eudyptula minor) are more likely to hunt cooperatively when foraging for aggregating prey than when prey are solitary [25], while instances of kleptoparasitism have been recorded by leopard seals (Hydrurga leptonyx) [26] and Hawaiian monk seals (Monachus schauinslandi) [27], which would otherwise go undetected through tracking data alone. Animal-borne cameras can also document cryptic behaviours [28], the use of tools [29], as well as mating in marine mammals [3032] and sharks [33], helping to identify possible breeding sites [34].

Basking sharks (Cetorhinus maximus) are the world’s second largest fish species [35] with a circumglobal distribution in temperate waters [36, 37]. They are susceptible to bycatch [38] and are Endangered (IUCN) throughout their range [39]. The northeast Atlantic is home to several internationally important conspicuous seasonal foraging aggregations, including the Isle of Man, the south west of England, the west of Ireland and the Sea of Hebrides on the west coast of Scotland [4044]. This last site has been subject to scientific study to improve the evidence base concerning a proposed Marine Protected Area (MPA), specifically intended for the protection of basking sharks, and also minke whales (Balaenoptera acutorostrata). Previous research efforts on basking sharks has advanced knowledge on their ecology, including foraging and diving activities [4547], energetic expenditure [48], habitat preference [49, 50], long distance and seasonal migration patterns [36, 41, 5153], site fidelity [41, 54, 55], and diel behaviour [56]. Comparatively, little is known of their reproductive biology, or the environmental or social context of sub-surface activity.

While basking sharks are likely to aggregate in the Sea of Hebrides to feed [43, 57], conspicuous behaviours not related to foraging have been observed such as nose-to-tail following, echelon swimming and breaching [43, 5759], which are thought to be associated with courtship displays as reported in number of elasmobranch species [6064]. There is no definite knowledge of the time spent by sharks interacting with conspecifics, whether this occurs sub-surface, and if courtship-like behaviour results in breeding attempts [49, 57, 65]. Such information would support previous findings of the importance of the Sea of Hebrides for basking sharks and aid the development of management plans within the proposed MPA. Animal-borne cameras therefore represent a promising tool to explore social interactions in a cryptic environment and were used in the present study to (i) investigate sub-surface behaviour, (ii) record interactions with conspecifics, (iii) improve knowledge on their fine scale habitat and depth use and (iv) reveal putative threats.

Materials and methods

Study site

All work was carried out in accordance with the UK HM Government Home Office under the Animals (Scientific Procedures) Act 1986 (Project Licence P23C6EFD) and under the Wildlife & Countryside Act 1981 (as amended) (Licence: 124812), and were reviewed and approved by the University of Exeter’s animal welfare and ethics review board (AWERB). Basking sharks were tagged with camera tags in the vicinity of the islands of Coll and Tiree within the Inner Hebrides, Scotland (N 56˚33’, W 6˚41’) in August 2018 and July 2019 (Fig 1A and 1B). Sharks were tagged by approaching them from behind using a 10 m vessel, until close enough to apply the camera tag system using a weighted 4 m long Hawaiian sling darting pole. Tags were attached to the base of the primary dorsal fin using a titanium dart (Wildlife Computers WA, USA). Sharks were sexed at the time of tagging using a sub-surface video camera system.

thumbnail
Fig 1. Study site and towed camera tags.

(A) Study site and tagging location of the six basking sharks (orange circles) within the Sea of the Hebrides Marine Protected Area (blue polygon) (B). (C) Towed camera technical drawing (not to scale) with a schematic of the rear and forward field of view (FOV) of the camera attached to 7m shark by a 1.8m tether in relation to the shark’s swimming speed. The FOV of a camera towed by shark swimming at 0.5 m.s-1 is represented in blue while the FOV when swimming at 2 m.s-1 is highlighted in red.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0253388.g001

Camera tags

Towed video camera tags (Fig 1C) comprised of four components: (i) a towed buoyant hydrodynamic body (CEiiA, Portugal) that encapsulated (ii) a camera system recording data with forward and rearward point-of-view (MrROV, UK), (iii) a G5 time-depth recorder (Cefas, UK) gathering information on temperature and depth at 1 Hz, and (iv) a SPOT6 satellite transmitter (Wildlife Computers, USA) communicating with the Argos System. The camera tags did not have lighting capability, which would have drawn on the battery, reduced the recording duration (instead relying on natural light during the daytime) and may have influenced the behaviour of the tagged animal or conspecifics. The combined package had a total weight of 1.3 kg in air, with a net buoyancy of 0.3 kg. Optimal towing dynamics of the system (e.g. attitude and stability) were derived for a scenario assuming a majority movement velocity of 2 m/sec (derived using electronic tags with integrated speed measurement [59]. Each tag system was attached to a titanium dart via a monofilament tether (1.8 m length in 2018; 1.5 m length in 2019), which comprised of a swivel and either an electronic Payload Release Device (PRD, Wildlife Computers, USA), enabling detachment of the tag from the shark at a pre-specified date and time (n = 5) or a manual galvanic corrosive link (model A2; International Fishing Devices, FL, USA; n = 1). In 2018, the PRDs used on two sharks, were programmed to detach at 06:00 BST the day following attachment and the galvanic release was estimated to provide a 12-hour attachment period given an approximate water temperature of 12°C. In 2019, PRDs were used on all deployments and programmed to release after a period of four days, which covered a period of forecasted poor weather and as such ensured cameras did not detach during periods when they could not be retrieved safely. Once the PRD activated, the camera tag detached from the dart, floated to the surface and position identified using the Argos System via the onboard SPOT6. Once the retrieval vessel was within 5 km of the most recent Argos location, a hand-held IC-R20 VHF receiver with directional antenna (ICOM, UK) and an RXG-234 goniometer (CLS, Toulouse, France) were used to aid retrieval. Towed cameras were programmed to record video data at 1080p resolution at 50 frames per second until either the memory or power were exhausted. Video recording was continuous in 2018, but a duty cycle of 30 seconds of recording every 5 minutes was used in 2019 to extend the recording duration. The 2018 continuous video data were therefore sub-sampled to 30-second video sequences every 5 minutes to match the duty cycling used in 2019. We also compared habitat use and behaviours revealed using the duty-cycled video with the continuous video data (collected in 2018), to understand what may have been missed in the duty-cycled data.

Satellite tracking data

Argos System location data were received during the study period, and were filtered to retain location classes (decreasing spatial accuracy) 3, 2, 1, A and B [66]. No location data were received during the camera attachment period for sharks tagged in 2018 as they were entirely subsurface for their respective deployment periods. Frequent location data were received for sharks 4, 5 and 6 (in 2019), however, during the camera attachment period. Feeding events and the presence of conspecifics observed in the video data were geolocated using the nearest Argos System locations received within 10 minutes of each event for these three sharks.

Data analysis

All video data were observed in VLC Media Player (Version 3.0.8). Initial assessment of all data was undertaken by viewing each 30-second video file at twice the native speed to ascertain information on the following: (i) ambient lighting conditions (more than 50% of video data gathered in dark conditions, termed ‘blackout’, separated into day and night) and (ii) seabed presence and habitat type (whether seabed was visible in more than 50% of data and dominant habitat type). Seabed habitats were classified following the European Nature Information System (EUNIS; https://eunis.eea.europa.eu) habitat classification system, and also included surface and mid-water swimming where neither the seabed nor surface were visible (S1 Table). The frequency and extent of other behaviours, including feeding behaviour (when the shark’s mouths was open for more than 50% of the video duration, i.e. >15 seconds), presence of conspecifics and other species were recorded, and species were identified to the finest possible taxonomic level. Where necessary, video data were watched at native or slowed to half speed to ascertain behaviour changes or other events.

To investigate the initial responses of basking sharks to tagging, Tail Beat Frequency (TBF) was calculated as the number of lateral undulatory movements visible in the video data. Using data collected in 2018, where video recording was continuous, TBF was estimated every minute (using 30 second of data), and every five minutes for the 2019 duty cycled data for the first 30 minutes following tagging. TBF was also estimated for each 30-second video sequence of surface feeding behaviour to compare swimming behaviour with previously reported swimming metrics from surface observations [67, 68]. Temperature and depth data collected by the time-depth recorder were analysed in R (version 3.5.3) for the duration that the cameras were recording.

Results

Basking sharks were instrumented with towed camera tags (2018: n = 2 females, n = 1 male; 2019: n = 2 males, n = 1 unidentified sex) ranging between 5 and 7 m in length. Tags were attached for a mean duration of 63.9 h ± 51.9 s.d. (range 7.8 to 111.1 h; 2018 mean 16.8 h ± 7.8 s.d.; 2019 mean 111.1 h ± 1.5 s.d.). Cameras recorded a cumulative 122.9 h of video data (mean 20.5 h ± 13.1 s.d., range 8.1 to 42.3 h), of which duty-cycled video represented a cumulative 20.8 h of data (mean: 3.5 h ± 3.1 s.d., range 0.8 to 8.5 h). All three rearward facing cameras failed to gather data in 2018 but were successful in 2019. On average (mean), 37% of video data (range 24% to 48%) were gathered at night, or while sharks were swimming at depths, when ambient light conditions were insufficient to observe the shark or its surrounding habitat (S2 Table).

Tagging response and recovery

The behavioural response to tagging was assessed using video and depth data gathered in the first 30 minutes of tag deployment. For sharks tagged in 2018, when cameras recorded continuously, raw video data were used. Sharks rapidly descended to the seabed following tagging to a mean depth of 28 m (± 20.2 s.d.; range 17.4 to 64 m) apart from shark 4 that descended to 2 m depth before returning to the surface to forage. After tagging, shark 1 swam to deeper waters remaining close to the seabed at least until 50 to 60 m depth below which ambient light conditions were too poor to validate whether the shark was near the seabed. Tail beat frequency and rate of turning indicated that there was considerable variation in the response of individual sharks to tagging (Fig 2). Shark 2 displayed the highest TBF within the first 5 minutes following tagging (1.1 Hz) before stabilising at 0.3 Hz. Shark 1 maintained the highest TBF over the 30 minute period (0.4 Hz) whereas shark 5 revealed the weakest response (0.2 Hz).

thumbnail
Fig 2. Response to tagging.

(A) Tail beat frequency in response to tagging calculated from 30 second segments of video data taken every minute for first 30 minutes of video data (uses continuously recorded data gathered in 2018 only). (B) Tail beat frequency calculated for each 30 second recording period every 5 minutes (for 30 minutes) for duty cycled camera tags deployed in 2019. Local order regression smoothing (colour lines; 0.5 span).

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0253388.g002

Vertical movement

Depth use varied between sharks, and between years (Fig 3). In 2018, sharks 1, 2 and 3 spent less than 1% of time swimming within a metre of the surface (0.15%, 0.38% and 0.13% of time respectively), while in 2019, sharks 4, 5 and 6 spent longer at the surface (49%, 30% and 15% respectively). Sharks tagged in 2018 swam at greater depth than those tagged in 2019 (mean in 2018 25.6 m ± 13.6, versus mean in 2019 13.6 m ± 3.5), with a maximum depth of 131 m recorded by shark 1. Sharks 1, 2 and 3 were in proximity to the seabed (seabed visible within video; e.g. Fig 3A–3C) for nearly half of the camera operation period (57%, 47% and 42% respectively of the duty cycled data, vs 61%, 48% and 48% respectively in the continuous data). When restricted to daylight hours, during which the vertical position of the shark in the water column could be characterised, sharks 1 to 3 were in proximity to the seabed 57%, 81% and 88% of camera recording time (in the duty cycled data, similar to the continuous data 61%, 82% and 92% respectively, Fig 3A–3C). Shark 1 made repeated short ascents to the surface before returning to 50 to 60 m depth where no behaviour could be inferred from the video data due to insufficient ambient light, whereas sharks 2 and 3 remained predominantly within the upper 30 m of the water column. Comparatively, sharks 4, 5 and 6 spent less than 20% of their camera recording duration near to the seafloor. These three sharks switched between intermittent surface swimming to repeated dives between the surface and the seafloor (“yo-yo” dives), interspersed with excursions to the seafloor (Fig 3D and 3E).

thumbnail
Fig 3. Depth use behaviour of basking sharks gathered from towed camera tags.

(A) Shark 1, (B) shark 2, and (C) shark 3 tagged in 2018, and (D) shark 4, (E) shark 5 and (F) shark 6 tagged in 2019. Periods of seabed (black bars) or surface swimming (red bars) when ambient light permitted habitat characterisation, where the top bars in A-C represent depth use inferred from duty-cycled video data, and lower bars continuous data. Tracking duration (h) shown in parentheses.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0253388.g003

Seabed habitat characterisation

Shark 1 spent 36% of daylight hours at depth where insufficient light levels prevented seabed habitat characterisation. When in sufficiently lit conditions, shark 1 was observed over three dominant seabed habitat types: i) infralittoral rock that includes bedrock and boulders with seaweed and kelp communities (EUNIS habitat code A3, 19% of time, S1 Table), ii) circalittoral rock including habitat dominated by macrofaunal communities such as echinoderms (A4, 19%), and iii) sublittoral sediments composed of sand, pebbles and mixed sediment (A5, 18%) (Fig 4A). Sharks 2 and 3 were observed predominantly over kelp, rock and seaweed habitat (A3, 34% of the recording duration each), and 12% and 6% of time over mixed sediment habitat (A5) respectively (Fig 4B and 4C). Shark 2 spent the most time (10%) of all three sharks tagged in 2018 swimming in the mid-water column. Continuous video data for the three sharks tagged in 2018 revealed similar habitat association, with less than 3% variation in habitat type compared to duty-cycled data (S1 Fig). Shark 4 tagged was only observed in proximity to the seabed on a single video recording over rocky substrate (A4) and spent 65% of the recording time at the surface (Fig 4D). Shark 5 swam over the largest range of seabed habitats (Fig 4E) and was associated most frequently with circalittoral habitats (A4, 12%). Shark 6 spent the most time of all sharks swimming mid-water (34% of recording duration), and predominantly associated with infralittoral rock dominated by seaweed (A3.215, 7%) (Fig 4F).

thumbnail
Fig 4. Habitat association by basking sharks.

Proportion of time sharks 1–6 (A to F) spent swimming over different habitat types characterised by their European Nature Information System (EUNIS) code (A3.125 mixed kelp with opportunistic red seaweed on sand-covered infralittoral rock, A3.212 Laminaria hyperborea on tide-swept infralittoral rock, A3.215 Dense foliose red seaweeds on silty moderately exposed infralittoral rock, A4.21 Echinoderms and crustose communities on circalittoral rock, A5.13 Infralittoral coarse sediment, A5.23 Infralittoral fine sand, A5.52 Kelp and seaweed communities on sublittoral sediment). Includes proportion of time sharks spent in the water column (No Vis. Seabed), or when habitat could not be characterised owing to poor light conditions from deep diving (Blackout Day) or recording at night (Blackout Night).

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0253388.g004

Surface foraging behaviour

None of the three sharks tagged in 2018 were observed feeding. By contrast, in 2019, a cumulative 120 minutes of feeding behaviour at the surface was captured (Fig 5). No sharks were observed feeding sub-surface. The time observed foraging varied between sharks, ranging from 23 to 51 minutes over 1.2 and 4.2 h time periods respectively (period between first and last video revealing foraging). Sharks 4, 5 and 6 were feeding for 35%, 26% and 21% of the camera operation duration respectively. Foraging sharks were usually within 1 m of the surface (average depth ± sd; shark 4: 0.3 m ± 0.2; shark 5: 0.9 ± 0.1; shark 6: 0.6 m ± 0.6) and feeding began before astronomical sunrise (earliest foraging behaviour recorded 04:40 BST), and continued for at least half an hour following astronomical sunset (latest foraging behaviour recorded 22:30 BST). Ambient light became too poor to identify feeding behaviour after 22:30 BST, however, based on time-depth recorder data, shark 5 remained at the surface until 22:55 BST before swimming to depth, so may have foraged during darkness. Sharks 4, 5 and 6 initiated feeding behaviour within 30 minutes of the camera deployment, with shark 4 returning to the surface in less than 20 seconds following tagging and was observed foraging in the first video segment 4 minutes into the deployment. The longest feeding interval was 3 h for shark 4, 3.1 h for shark 5 and 1.5 h for shark 6. Sharks were recorded swallowing 16 times throughout feeding events (3, 6 and 7 times for sharks 4 to 6 respectively). Feeding behaviour represented three quarters of the duty-cycled video data gathered at the surface. If sharks allocated this proportion of time to feeding when swimming within 1 m of the surface, sharks 4, 5 and 6 would have fed for 4.6, 7.9 and 11.3 h of their camera deployment duration respectively. TBF of sharks while feeding was 0.20 Hz ± 0.02 for shark 6, 0.21 Hz ± 0.02 for shark 5, and 0.25 Hz ± 0.02 for shark 4. Sharks maintained the same TBF regardless of whether they were foraging or not (Wilcoxon signed rank test V = 853, p = 0.49, foraging 0.22 Hz ± 0.03 versus mouth closed 0.22 Hz ± 0.04), or when being in proximity to the seabed (TBF 0.24 Hz ± 0.08, range 0.17 Hz to 0.38 Hz) compared to the surface (0.22 Hz ± 0.02, range 0.19 Hz to 0.25 Hz). The later comparison could not be tested statistically as only two sharks (sharks 5 and 6) were recorded both at the surface and in proximity to the seafloor.

thumbnail
Fig 5. Basking shark satellite tracking during camera tag attachment.

(A) Satellite tracking locations for shark 4 (green), shark 5 (orange) and shark 6 (red) during camera tag attachment periods in 2019. Location of feeding events for each shark (squares in corresponding colours). Selected behaviours are highlighted with given locations (B-G). In (B) to (E) shark 4 is seen feeding at the surface following a second male shark. Shark 5 camera captures a second shark swimming slowly over the seabed in (F) as it swims towards the surface. (G) Shark 6 feeding at the surface.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0253388.g005

Conspecifics

Five towed camera tags recorded other basking sharks on eleven separate occasions, of which six were during surface foraging events (sharks 4 to 6), with the longest interaction lasting 2 h 15 minutes during which shark 4 was following a feeding male shark (Fig 5B–5E and Fig 6E). Sharks maintained similar TBF swimming at the surface regardless of whether in the presence of conspecifics (0.23 Hz ± 0.02), feeding (0.22 Hz) or not (0.22 Hz ± 0.04). The camera tags did not directly reveal mating but did record some courtship-like behaviours and early morning group behaviour. Shark 5 recorded another shark foraging ~15 m away at the surface (Fig 5D), which then stopped feeding and closely approached the tagged shark (within 2 m), switching from its left side to its right by swimming underneath shark 5 before swimming alongside it. Shark 4 followed a second shark very closely (<1 m) at speed (TBF 0.33 Hz) at 60 m depth. The other shark was then observed approaching the caudal fin of shark 4 perpendicularly within 1 m, turned to swim beneath it, and followed shark 4 from below. In the subsequent video recording, recorded five minutes later, shark 4 was observed close following the second shark nose-to-tail at speed at 29 m depth. A second shark was observed in the continuous video data of shark 1 swimming ~ 20 m ahead and above the tagged sharked at 49 m depth. The second shark was only visible for < 5 secs due to poor visibility at depth. TBF could not be measured owing to instability of the camera. Group behaviour was also observed; shark 3 was observed swimming slowly (TBF: 0.17 Hz, slower than average swimming speed 0.22 Hz) near to the seabed (18–25 m depth) within 5 m of at least nine other basking sharks in the early morning (05:11 to 06:05 BST 3-Aug-2018). The sharks swam closely enough that they touched body and fins two and three times respectively, and swam both closely next to one another and in an echelon formation. Shark 3 was observed swimming ~1 m directly above a second shark in the same direction for 25 seconds before swimming away and following a third shark nose-to-tail, while a fourth and fifth shark swam over the tagged shark, bumping into the camera. In the continuous video data of shark 3, a three minute clip revealed at least nine individual sharks, with an average of four sharks (± 1.9 sharks) sighted per 3 minute clip between 05:11 to 06:05 compared to two sharks (± 0.9, max 4 sharks) over the same time period for the 30 second duty-cycled clips. This is the first evidence to our knowledge of basking sharks aggregating near the seabed (Fig 6E, Shark 5 was observed making an abrupt 4 minute duration dive from feeding at the surface to the descending to the seabed at 46 m depth at speed where the rearward camera captures a second shark swimming slowly over the seabed (Fig 5D) and the tagged shark ascends back to the surface with a tail beat frequency of 0.23 Hz.

thumbnail
Fig 6. Observed behaviours from towed camera tags.

(A) Shark 3 moving over sandy seabed, (B) shark 4 feeding while following a second feeding shark at the surface, (C) shark 2 moving over mixed ground with seaweeds (D) shark 5 trailed closely by a second shark, (E) shark 3 engaged in early morning group behaviour—three sharks visible; instrumented shark (centre) and two other sharks (far left and top right), (F) shark 4 rearward camera obscured by plastic sheet, (G) shark 5 rearward camera revealing defecation, and (H) shark 5 rearward camera revealing small horse mackerel following the tagged shark.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0253388.g006

Breaching and defecation

One shark breached clear of the water during recording (Fig 7) at 19:22 BST on the 2-Aug-2018, ascending from 72 m deep at approximately 0.9 m s-1 of vertical gain, reaching the surface in 77 seconds The shark exited the water completely before descending to similar depth again (74 m) within 61 seconds at a vertical speed of 1.2 m s-1. Five defecation events were recorded in two sharks by the rearward facing cameras, one by shark 4 at the surface, and four times by shark 5 on average 6.1 h apart (± 3.7 s.d., range 1.8 to 8.3 h), twice when near to the seabed (43 and 19 m respectively), and twice mid-water between 10 and 15 m deep (Fig 6G).

thumbnail
Fig 7. Basking shark breaching.

Breach recorded by a towed camera tag. (A) The shark starts to ascend from 72 m depth at approximately 0.94 m s-1 of vertical gain, reaching the surface (in view, B) in 77 seconds The shark can be seen completely out of the water (C), before descending (D) to depth again. The timing and depth associated with each image (A-D) are identified on the breaching depth profile (E).

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0253388.g007

Other species

Video cameras recorded ctenophores, moon jellies (Aurelia aurita) and lion’s mane (Cyanea capillata) jellyfish throughout the water column, species that are consistent for the region [69], with no evidence of sharks either avoiding or eating them. Sharks swam over benthic sessile organisms including sea urchins, brittle and common starfish, sea cucumber, and soft coral (alcyonium). With the exception of shark 6, all sharks had between one and three lamprey attached between the anal fins, as well as behind the dorsal fin in the case of shark 4. All sharks were followed at some point (between 1 and 13% of the video data) by small fish (Fig 6H), likely Atlantic horse mackerel (Trachurus trachurus), throughout the water column and near to the seafloor, most commonly over rocky substrate. These fish were cleaning the sharks (or themselves) in over half of the observations (up to 73% (n = 95) for shark 6), and shoaled around the shark’s body or trailing their tail (recorded in rearward cameras of sharks 4, 5 and 6) in shoals up to hundreds of fish, although in the majority (80%) of video data there were less than 100 fish. Two instances of antagonistic behaviour occurred, in which sharks 5 and 6 snapped their jaws and made abrupt tight turns of their head in response to shoals of Atlantic horse mackerel appearing to bite the shark caudal fin, followed by three rapid tail beats, dispersing the fish.

Marine debris

Camera tags also provided incidental evidence of marine debris. Fishing line was observed floating in the water column close to sharks on three separate occasions (shark 5 n = 1, shark 6 n = 2), while a plastic sheet became attached to shark 4’s towed camera 35 minutes into the deployment (Fig 6F), obscuring part of the rear view until the end the deployment (11.7 h later).

Differences between continuous and duty-cycled data

There were few differences in habitat association and behaviours observed in the duty-cycled data compared to the continuous data for sharks 1, 2 and 3. The proportion of time allocated to different habitat types varied by less than 3%. Only three behaviours were missed due to duty-cycling: the sighting of a second shark for 5 seconds by shark 1, two fin touching events during the grouping behaviour at depth by shark 3, and the breach, which was originally identified from the depth profile. The number of sightings of other species remained similar between recording schedules, except for intermittent following of trailing fish which were reported up to four times more frequently in the continuous data (14% compared to 3% of clips for shark 3). Most of the basking sharks’ noteworthy behaviours such as feeding, trailing or being followed by conspecifics were sustained over periods longer than the four minutes 30 seconds period between video segments. The sub-sampled video data revealed the grouping behaviour over the same overall time scale as the continuous data, but less than half as many sharks were recorded on average (2.1 compared to 4.3), and less than half the maximum number sharks (4 compared to 9) were identified during a single video segment.

Discussion

Overview

This study represents the first long-term deployment of animal-borne cameras on basking sharks recording a cumulative 123 hours of video data and revealed the first evidence of sub-surface interaction with conspecifics. Cameras provided unprecedented insight into the social and environmental context of basking shark behaviour, which was previously limited to surface observations, as well as their fine scale habitat association at depth. While basking sharks are thought to aggregate in Scottish waters to feed [50, 57], the sharks in this study spent a surprising proportion of the study period in proximity to the seabed (up to 88% of recordings made in daylight hours) where sharks swam predominantly over infralittoral rock. Knowledge of habitat association is valuable for evaluating the effects of human activity upon the species (e.g. fishing, aquaculture, wild harvesting of natural products, civil engineering, military activities). Any future threat assessments of this species should explicitly consider depth behaviour and seabed habitat associations given the time sharks allocated to swimming near to the seafloor, which increases the possible risk of overlap with human activity sub-surface. For example, in New Zealand, the greatest proportion of accidental capture of basking sharks were by trawlers near or on the seafloor [38]. Towed cameras confirmed the presence of marine debris, including fishing gear, highlighting possible risks of ingestion or entanglement [70, 71]; however, the Inner Hebrides experiences some of lowest levels of pollution from marine waste in the UK [72]. The extent of association with the seabed also has implications for conducting population estimates, since these are based primarily on surface observations [73]. The number of sharks in the region may be underestimated owing to the rather limited time spent at the surface or even in the mid-water column.

Although the tracking duration was too short to robustly test diel patterns of depth use, all sharks swam deeper at night than during the day, supporting previous findings of reverse vertical diel migration in basking sharks in inner shelf areas near thermal fronts [47, 56, 74]. Sharks may have followed the vertical diel migration of prey at night and fed at depth [45, 53], however, ambient light conditions were too dark to identify possible foraging behaviour, and no feeding behaviour was captured even when sharks were discernible on the seabed at dawn. Sharks made intermittent oscillatory dives to the seabed between foraging events, typified by shark 4. While these dives are likely to have multiple functions in sharks [7578], yo-yo type dives have been suggested to play a role in prey searching [45, 76] as an optimal foraging strategy to maximise encounter rate with spatially and temporally distributed prey such as zooplankton [79]. However, feeding behaviour was only observed at the surface and only for three sharks tagged in 2019. Sharks maintained similar tail beat frequencies when swimming at the surface regardless of whether they were feeding or not and were similar to previously reported boat-based observation [67]. As ram ventilating species, basking sharks may need to maintain a minimum speed to optimise water flow across the gills to meet oxygen requirements [80], possibly compensating for the high drag associated with filter feeding at the surface. Animal-borne cameras can therefore reveal aspects of basking shark feeding ecology, such as digestion, which may be challenging or impossible to infer from conventional tracking technologies. In the present study, although video data collected was duty-cycled, prey swallowing and defecation events were recorded. If combined with feeding rates, local prey concentration and daily energy expenditure, more accurate energetic models could be populated for basking sharks. While it has been suggested that marine mammals and seabirds play a role in enhancing primary productivity and stimulating carbon export [8183], the biogeochemical role of basking sharks in the oceans has not been explored. The towed cameras revealed that the sharks defecated at the surface as well as on the seabed, possibly enhancing primary production at depth through fertilisation. While basking sharks are thought to aggregate in the region to feed, sharks fed less than might be expected, with none of the sharks tagged in 2018 feeding in over 16 hours of cumulative video data. Owing to the energetic costs associated with filter feeding, which may be up to twice the costs associated with routine activity at similar speeds [84, 85], basking sharks may have not been feeding because of low prey densities in 2018 at which sharks would not achieve net energy gain [86].

Tagged sharks recorded the presence of conspecifics on eleven separate occasions. Conspecifics were predominantly observed at the surface during feeding events either following or leading the tagged shark closely. While close following is likely to provide a hydrodynamic advantage to the following shark by conserving energy associated with drag or possibly help capture prey missed by the leading shark [57], it has also been suggested to play a role in pre-courtship display [57, 87, 88]. In this study, a female shark was observed swimming towards a male shark from a distance, exhibiting nose-to-tail following, stacking behaviour (swimming below the tagged shark) and parallel swimming while shark 5 was not feeding. Intentional swimming was also recorded by Gore et al. [57], however this type of behaviour was more rarely recorded compared to other close following behaviours. While similar social behaviour has been reported in aggregating sharks at the surface [8789], this study reported the first sub-surface putative courtship displays for basking sharks. At least nine sharks were observed close following and swimming parallel to shark 3 near to the seabed at dawn, presenting the first evidence of basking shark aggregations at depth. Social groups of sharks are thought to be very short term and sporadic, and may play a role in finding breeding partners [65], particularly in usually solitary sharks such as basking sharks that may use summer aggregations as an opportunity to find mates [87], with high relative population densities possibly triggering mating behaviour in schooling species [90]. Since the tagged sharks maintained similar tailbeat frequencies regardless of being in the presence of conspecifics or alone, both at the surface and at depth, interactions with conspecifics may be missed by using conventional tracking technologies. While no mating behaviour was directly recorded, this study reports previously unknown sub-surface putative courtship displays, highlighting the importance of the region to basking sharks.

Cameras provided unprecedented social context to rare behaviours such as two fast burst chasing events at depth associated with sharks 4 and 5 closely following, or being followed, by a second shark. One chase event lasted over five minutes perhaps suggesting more than an aggressive response. In addition, a complete breach revealing ascent and descent performed by a basking shark was recorded onboard the animal in the present study. Shark 1 was observed to ascend rapidly from the seafloor before clearing the surface and descending back to similar depths. Breaching is observed in a range of species [63, 9193] and has been associated with communication [94], mate finding [95], predatory ambush [96] and even fun [97]. Because breaching has been estimated to be relatively energetically demanding [48, 59, 93, 98], it is likely to have a fitness benefit [99]. While the function of breaching remains unclear, it may be related to social behaviour [48, 57, 65]. Breaching has been observed in areas where surface activity, such as close following, has been recorded [57, 87].

With all tagging studies, it is essential to consider the effect of tags on the animal. In this study the response to tagging was measured, with sharks displaying an initial tail beat frequency up to three times higher than the relative baseline frequency. Within 15 minutes (mean 10 minutes), tail beat declined and stabilised, remaining similar throughout the camera operation period. To achieve minimum burden of animal-borne cameras, a trade-off exists with device size, and battery life and/or storage. Several methods have been designed to extend the deployment duration and likelihood of capturing behaviours of interest such as sampling triggered by other sensors (such as depth recorders [100] and accelerometers [19], or selecting sampling rates that are biologically relevant to the tagged species. Hooker et al. [100] suggested that when the temporal quantification of a behaviour is important, the sampling rate must be more frequent than the duration of the event, while if investigating the frequency of a behaviour, duty-cycling must take place at intervals shorter than the duration of the behaviour. Basking sharks are slow swimming (mean speed 1 km.h-1 [59]) filter feeders with behaviours such as interactions with conspecifics and foraging lasting several minutes to hours. In the present study, camera tags were duty-cycled, sampling 30 seconds every 5 minutes, which enabled the recordings to be extended by about 100% compared with continuous filming (mean 2018 continuous video data 12.6 h ± 4.2, compared to mean 2019 video data 28.3 h ± 15.2). Sub-sampling the 2018 continuous data to match the duty-cycled 2019 footage revealed little variation in habitat association and suggested that duty cycling meant only few incidents were missed. Events such as feeding, defecation, breaching or short-lived interactions between conspecifics were sufficiently sporadic that they would likely only be captured if recording frequency was sufficiently long. While the duty-cycled video data highlighted a similar frequency of common behaviours to the continuous video data, the sampling frequency and interval chosen here are unlikely to be suitable for more dynamic species, such as ambush predators with burst behaviours, low prey encounter rates and short handling times, like the great white shark. Future deployments of cameras with other sensors, such as accelerometers or oceanographic sensors, would also provide important visual context to tracking data. Cameras could also be used to investigate anthropogenic effects in the marine environment on animals at liberty, by directly identifying threats as well as subsequent behavioural responses, which may not be obvious from other types of biologging sensors [101].

Supporting information

S1 Fig. Comparison of habitat association by basking sharks.

Proportion of time sharks 1–3 spent swimming over different habitat types characterised by their European Nature Information System (EUNIS) code (A3.125 mixed kelp with opportunistic red seaweed on sand-covered infralittoral rock, A3.212 Laminaria hyperborea on tide-swept infralittoral rock, A3.215 Dense foliose red seaweeds on silty moderately exposed infralittoral rock, A4.21 Echinoderms and crustose communities on circalittoral rock, A5.13 Infralittoral coarse sediment, A5.23 Infralittoral fine sand, A5.52 Kelp and seaweed communities on sublittoral sediment) derived from duty-cycled data (A-C) and continuous data (D-F). Includes proportion of time sharks spent in the water column (No Vis. Seabed), or when habitat could not be characterised owing to poor light conditions from deep diving (Blackout Day) or recording at night (Blackout Night).

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0253388.s001

(TIF)

S1 Table. Summary table of the European Nature Information System (EUNIS) habitat codes used to classify the habitat types used by the basking sharks.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0253388.s002

(DOCX)

S2 Table. Summary information on deployment of towed camera tags (2018 and 2019), including locations of deployment, tag detachment and retrieval times, attachment and data duration and camera performance.

Statistics for sharks 1 to 3 are derived from sub-sampled video data to match the format of duty cycled tags deployed in 2019.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0253388.s003

(DOCX)

Acknowledgments

The authors wish to thank the boat operators, skippers and crew of the Bold Ranger including J. Fairbairns, R. Darby, I. Malcolm, H. Mansfield and R.A. McCann.

References

  1. 1. Cooke SJ, Hinch SG, Wikelski M, Andrews RD, Kuchel LJ, Wolcott TG, et al. Biotelemetry: a mechanistic approach to ecology. Trends Ecol Evol. 2004 Jun 1;19(6):334–43. pmid:16701280
  2. 2. Rutz C, Hays GC. New frontiers in biologging science. Biol Lett. 2009 11 March;5: 289–292. pmid:19324624
  3. 3. Hebblewhite M, Haydon DT. Distinguishing technology from biology: a critical review of the use of GPS telemetry data in ecology. Philos Trans R Soc Lond, B Biol. Sci. 2010 Jul 27;365(1550):2303–12. pmid:20566506
  4. 4. Hussey NE, Kessel ST, Aarestrup K, Cooke SJ, Cowley PD, Fisk AT, et al. Aquatic animal telemetry: a panoramic window into the underwater world. Science. 2015 Jun 12;348(6240):1255642. pmid:26068859
  5. 5. Kays R, Crofoot MC, Jetz W, Wikelski M. Terrestrial animal tracking as an eye on life and planet. Science. 2015 Jun 12;348(6240):aaa2478. pmid:26068858
  6. 6. Wilmers CC, Nickel B, Bryce CM, Smith JA, Wheat RE, Yovovich V. The golden age of bio‐logging: how animal‐borne sensors are advancing the frontiers of ecology. Ecol. 2015 Jul 1;96(7):1741–53. pmid:26378296
  7. 7. Seminoff JA, Jones TT, Marshall GJ. Underwater behaviour of green turtles monitored with video-time-depth recorders: what’s missing from dive profiles?. Mar Ecol Prog Ser. 2006 Sep 20;322:269–80.
  8. 8. Votier SC, Bicknell A, Cox SL, Scales KL, Patrick SC. A bird’s eye view of discard reforms: bird-borne cameras reveal seabird/fishery interactions. PLoS One. 2013;8(3). pmid:23483906
  9. 9. Gradenwitz A. Pigeons as picture-makers. Technical World Magazine. 1908;10:485–7.
  10. 10. Carroll G, Slip D, Jonsen I, Harcourt R. Supervised accelerometry analysis can identify prey capture by penguins at sea. J Exp Biol. 2014 Dec 15;217(24):4295–302. pmid:25394635
  11. 11. Nakamura I, Meyer CG, Sato K. Unexpected positive buoyancy in deep sea sharks, Hexanchus griseus, and a Echinorhinus cookei. PloS One. 2015 Jun 10;10(6):e0127667. pmid:26061525
  12. 12. Andrzejaczek S, Gleiss AC, Lear KO, Pattiaratchi CB, Chapple T, Meekan M. Biologging tags reveal links between fine-scale horizontal and vertical movement behaviours in tiger sharks (Galeocerdo cuvier). Front Mar Sci. 2019;6:229.
  13. 13. Papastamatiou YP, Watanabe YY, Bradley D, Dee LE, Weng K, Lowe CG, et al. Drivers of daily routines in an ectothermic marine predator: hunt warm, rest warmer?. PLoS One. 2015;10(6). pmid:26061229
  14. 14. Jewell OJ, Gleiss AC, Jorgensen SJ, Andrzejaczek S, Moxley JH, Beatty SJ, et al. Cryptic habitat use of white sharks in kelp forest revealed by animal-borne video. Biol Lett. 2019 Apr 26;15(4):20190085. pmid:30940023
  15. 15. Heithaus MR, McLash JJ, Frid A, Dill LM, Marshall GJ. Novel insights into green sea turtle behaviour using animal-borne video cameras. J Mar Biol Assoc U. K. 2002 Dec;82(6):1049–50.
  16. 16. Takahashi A, Kokubun N, Mori Y, Shin HC. Krill-feeding behaviour of gentoo penguins as shown by animal-borne camera loggers. Polar Biol. 2008 Sep 1;31(10):1291–4.
  17. 17. Loyd KA, Hernandez SM, Carroll JP, Abernathy KJ, Marshall GJ. Quantifying free-roaming domestic cat predation using animal-borne video cameras. Biol Conserv. 2013 Apr 1;160:183–9.
  18. 18. Nakamura I, Goto Y, Sato K. Ocean sunfish rewarm at the surface after deep excursions to forage for siphonophores. J Anim Ecol. 2015 May 1;84(3):590–603. pmid:25643743
  19. 19. Yoshino K, Takahashi A, Adachi T, Costa DP, Robinson PW, Peterson SH, et al. Acceleration-triggered animal-borne videos show a dominance of fish in the diet of female northern elephant seals. J Exp Biol. 2020 Mar 1;223(5). pmid:32041802
  20. 20. Watanabe YY, Takahashi A. Linking animal-borne video to accelerometers reveals prey capture variability. PNAS. 2013 Feb 5;110(6):2199–204. pmid:23341596
  21. 21. Watanabe YY, Ito M, Takahashi A. Testing optimal foraging theory in a penguin–krill system. Proc R Soc. 2014 Mar 22;281(1779):20132376. pmid:24478293
  22. 22. Narazaki T, Sato K, Abernathy KJ, Marshall GJ, Miyazaki N. Loggerhead turtles (Caretta caretta) use vision to forage on gelatinous prey in mid-water. PLoS One. 2013;8(6). pmid:23776603
  23. 23. Kane SA, Fulton AH, Rosenthal LJ. When hawks attack: animal-borne video studies of goshawk pursuit and prey-evasion strategies. J Exp B. 2015 Jan 15;218(2):212–22. pmid:25609783
  24. 24. Heaslip SG, Bowen WD, Iverson SJ. Testing predictions of optimal diving theory using animal-borne video from harbour seals (Phoca vitulina concolor). Can J Zool. 2014;92(4):309–18.
  25. 25. Sutton GJ, Hoskins AJ, Arnould JP. Benefits of group foraging depend on prey type in a small marine predator, the little penguin. PloS One. 2015;10(12). pmid:26674073
  26. 26. Krause DJ, Goebel ME, Marshall GJ, Abernathy K. Novel foraging strategies observed in a growing leopard seal (Hydrurga leptonyx) population at Livingston Island, Antarctic Peninsula. Anim Biotelem. 2015 Dec 1;3(1):24.
  27. 27. Parrish FA, Marshall GJ, Buhleier B, Antonelis GA. Foraging interaction between monk seals and large predatory fish in the Northwestern Hawaiian Islands. Endanger Species Res. 2008 May 16;4(3):299–308.
  28. 28. Tackaberry JE, Cade DE, Goldbogen JA, Wiley DN, Friedlaender AS, Stimpert AK. From a calf’s perspective: humpback whale nursing behavior on two US feeding grounds. PeerJ. 2020 Mar 4;8:e8538. pmid:32181052
  29. 29. Rutz C, Bluff LA, Weir AA, Kacelnik A. Video cameras on wild birds. Science. 2007 Nov 2;318(5851):765–765. pmid:17916693
  30. 30. Boness DJ, Bowen WD, Buhleier BM, Marshall GJ. Mating tactics and mating system of an aquatic-mating pinniped: the harbor seal, Phoca vitulina. Behav Ecol Sociobiol. 2006 Nov 1;61(1):119–30.
  31. 31. Herman EY, Herman LM, Pack AA, Marshall G, Shepard MC, Bakhtiari M. When whales collide: Crittercam offers insight into the competitive behavior of humpback whales on their Hawaiian wintering grounds. Mar Technol Soc J. 2007 Dec 1;41(4):35–43.
  32. 32. Oña J, Duque E, Garland EC, Seger K, Narváez M, Maldonado J, et al. Giant’s Dance: Underwater Social and Vocal Behavior of Humpback Whales (Megaptera novaeangliae) Recorded on the Northern Coast of Ecuador. Aquat Mamm. 2019;45(4):456–64.
  33. 33. Meyer CG, Anderson JM, Coffey DM, Hutchinson MR, Royer MA, Holland KN. Habitat geography around Hawaii’s oceanic islands influences tiger shark (Galeocerdo cuvier) spatial behaviour and shark bite risk at ocean recreation sites. Sci Rep. 2018 Mar 21;8(1):1–8. pmid:29311619
  34. 34. Papastamatiou YP, Meyer CG, Watanabe YY, Heithaus MR. Animal‐borne video cameras and their use to study shark ecology and conservation. Shark Research: Emerging Technologies and Applications for the Field and Laboratory. 2018 Sep 3:83–91.
  35. 35. Sims DW. Sieving a living: a review of the biology, ecology and conservation status of the plankton‐feeding basking shark Cetorhinus maximus. Adv Mar Biol. 2008 Jan 1;54:171–220. pmid:18929065
  36. 36. Braun CD, Skomal GB, Thorrold SR. Integrating archival tag data and a high-resolution oceanographic model to estimate basking shark (Cetorhinus maximus) movements in the Western Atlantic. Front Mar Sci. 2018 Feb 8;5:25.
  37. 37. Dewar H, Wilson SG, Hyde JR, Snodgrass OE, Leising A, Lam CH, et al. Basking Shark (Cetorhinus maximus) Movements in the Eastern North Pacific Determined Using Satellite Telemetry. Front Mar Sci. 2018 May 16;5:163.
  38. 38. Francis M, Duffy C. Distribution, seasonal abundance and bycatch of basking sharks (Cetorhinus maximus) in New Zealand, with observations on their winter habitat. Mar Biol. 2002 Apr 1;140(4):831–42.
  39. 39. Rigby CL, Barreto R, Carlson J, Fernando D, Fordham S, Francis MP, et al. Cetorhinus maximus (errata version published in 2020). The IUCN Red List of Threatened Species 2019: e.T4292A166822294. [cited 2014 May 14]. Available from https://dx.doi.org/10.2305/IUCN.UK.2019-3.RLTS.T4292A166822294.en
  40. 40. Berrow SD, Heardman C. The basking shark Cetorhinus maximus (Gunnerus) in Irish waters: patterns of distribution and abundance. In Biology and Environment: Proceedings of the Royal Irish Academy 1994 Oct 1 (pp. 101–107). Royal Irish Academy. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1749-6632.1994.tb44420.x pmid:7847681
  41. 41. Dolton HR, Gell FR, Hall J, Hall G, Hawkes LA, Witt MJ. Assessing the importance of Isle of Man waters for the basking shark Cetorhinus maximus. Endanger Species Rese. 2020 Feb 13;41:209–23.
  42. 42. Southall EJ, Sims DW, Metcalfe JD, Doyle JI, Fanshawe S, Lacey C, et al. Spatial distribution patterns of basking sharks on the European shelf: preliminary comparison of satellite-tag geolocation, survey and public sightings data. Mar Biol Assoc U.K. J Mar Biol Assoc U.K. 2005 Oct 1;85(5):1083.
  43. 43. Speedie CD, Johnson LA, Witt MJ. 2009 Basking Shark Hotspots on the West Coast of Scotland: Key sites, threats and implications for conservation of the species. Commissioned Report No. 339. Scottish Natural Heritage, Inverness.
  44. 44. Witt MJ, Hardy T, Johnson L, McClellan CM, Pikesley SK, Ranger S, et al. Basking sharks in the northeast Atlantic: spatio-temporal trends from sightings in UK waters. Mar Ecol Prog Ser. 2012 Jul 12;459:121–34.
  45. 45. Gore MA, Rowat D, Hall J, Gell FR, Ormond RF. Transatlantic migration and deep mid-ocean diving by basking shark. Biol Lett. 2008 Aug 23;4(4):395–8. pmid:18511407
  46. 46. Queiroz N, Vila-Pouca C, Couto A, Southall EJ, Mucientes G, Humphries NE, et al. Convergent foraging tactics of marine predators with different feeding strategies across heterogeneous ocean environments. Front Mar Sci. 2017 Aug 2;4:239.
  47. 47. Doherty PD, Baxter JM, Godley BJ, Graham RT, Hall G, Hall J, et al. Seasonal changes in basking shark vertical space use in the north-east Atlantic. Mar Biol. 2019 Oct 1;166(10):129.
  48. 48. Johnston EM, Halsey LG, Payne NL, Kock AA, Iosilevskii G, Whelan B, et al. Latent power of basking sharks revealed by exceptional breaching events. Biol Lett. 2018 Sep 30;14(9):20180537. pmid:30209042
  49. 49. Miller PI, Scales KL, Ingram SN, Southall EJ, Sims DW. Basking sharks and oceanographic fronts: quantifying associations in the north‐east Atlantic. Funct Ecol. 2015 Aug;29(8):1099–109. 0.1111/1365-2435.12423.
  50. 50. Austin RA, Hawkes LA, Doherty PD, Henderson SM, Inger R, Johnson L, et al. Predicting habitat suitability for basking sharks (Cetorhinus maximus) in UK waters using ensemble ecological niche modelling. J Sea Res. 2019 Nov 1;153:101767.
  51. 51. Doherty PD, Baxter JM, Gell FR, Godley BJ, Graham RT, Hall G, et al. Long-term satellite tracking reveals variable seasonal migration strategies of basking sharks in the north-east Atlantic. Sci Rep. 2017 Feb 20;7:42837. pmid:28216646
  52. 52. Skomal GB, Zeeman SI, Chisholm JH, Summers EL, Walsh HJ, McMahon KW, et al. Transequatorial migrations by basking sharks in the western Atlantic Ocean. Curr Biol. 2009 Jun 23;19(12):1019–22. pmid:19427211
  53. 53. Sims DW, Southall EJ, Richardson AJ, Reid PC, Metcalfe JD. Seasonal movements and behaviour of basking sharks from archival tagging: no evidence of winter hibernation. Mar Ecol Prog Ser. 2003 Feb 20;248:187–96.
  54. 54. Johnston EM, Mayo PA, Mensink PJ, Savetsky E, Houghton JD. Serendipitous re‐sighting of a basking shark Cetorhinus maximus reveals inter‐annual connectivity between American and European coastal hotspots. J Fish Biol. 2019 Dec;95(6):1530–4. pmid:31621067
  55. 55. Doherty PD, Baxter JM, Godley BJ, Graham RT, Hall G, Hall J, et al. Testing the boundaries: seasonal residency and inter-annual site fidelity of basking sharks in a proposed marine protected area. Biol Conserv. 2017 May 1;209:68–75.
  56. 56. Shepard EL, Ahmed MZ, Southall EJ, Witt MJ, Metcalfe JD, Sims DW. Diel and tidal rhythms in diving behaviour of pelagic sharks identified by signal processing of archival tagging data. Mar Ecol Prog Ser. 2006 Dec 20;328:205–13. pmid:31775150
  57. 57. Gore M, Abels L, Wasik S, Saddler L, Ormond R. Are close-following and breaching behaviours by basking sharks at aggregation sites related to courtship?. J Mar Biol Assoc UK. 2019 May;99(3):681–93.
  58. 58. Matthews LH. Reproduction in the basking shark, Cetorhinus maximus (Gunner). Philos Trans R Soc Lond B, Biol Sci. 1950 Apr 5;234(612):247–316. pmid:24537280
  59. 59. Rudd JL, Exeter OM, Hall J, Hall G, Henderson SM, Kerry C, et al. High resolution biologging of breaching by the world’s second largest fish. Sci Rep. 2021 Mar 4;11(1):1–5. pmid:33414495
  60. 60. Klimley AP, Pyle P, Anderson SD. The behavior of white sharks and their pinniped prey during predatory attacks. In Great White Sharks 1996 Jan 1 (pp. 175–191). Academic Press.
  61. 61. Pratt HL, Carrier JC. A review of elasmobranch reproductive behavior with a case study on the nurse shark, Ginglymostoma cirratum. Environ Biol Fishes. 2001 Feb 1;60(1–3):157–88.
  62. 62. Marshall AD, Bennett MB. Reproductive ecology of the reef manta ray Manta alfredi in southern Mozambique. J Fish Biol. 2010 Jul;77(1):169–90. pmid:20646146
  63. 63. Curtis TH, Macesic LJ. Observations of breaching behavior in juvenile Bull Sharks, Carcharhinus leucas. Florida Scientist. 2011 Oct 1:253–7.
  64. 64. Stevens GM, Hawkins JP, Roberts CM. Courtship and mating behaviour of manta rays Mobula alfredi and M. birostris in the Maldives. J Fish Biol. 2018 Aug;93(2):344–59. pmid:30066396
  65. 65. Jacoby DM, Croft DP, Sims DW. Social behaviour in sharks and rays: analysis, patterns and implications for conservation. Fish Fish. 2012 Dec;13(4):399–417.
  66. 66. Witt MJ, Åkesson S, Broderick AC, Coyne MS, Ellick J, Formia A, et al. Assessing accuracy and utility of satellite-tracking data using Argos-linked Fastloc-GPS. Anim Behav. 2010 Sep 1;80(3):571.
  67. 67. Sims DW. Filter-feeding and cruising swimming speeds of basking sharks compared with optimal models: they filter-feed slower than predicted for their size. J Experimental Mar Biol Ecol. 2000 Jun 1;249(1):65–76. pmid:10817828
  68. 68. Watanabe YY, Lydersen C, Fisk AT, Kovacs KM. The slowest fish: swim speed and tail-beat frequency of Greenland sharks. J Exp Mari Biol Ecol. 2012 Sep 1;426:5–11.
  69. 69. Pikesley SK, Godley BJ, Ranger S, Richardson PB, Witt MJ. Cnidaria in UK coastal waters: description of spatio-temporal patterns and inter-annual variability. J Mar Biol Assoc. 2014. 94:1401–1408.
  70. 70. Fossi MC, Coppola D, Baini M, Giannetti M, Guerranti C, Marsili L, et al. Large filter feeding marine organisms as indicators of microplastic in the pelagic environment: the case studies of the Mediterranean basking shark (Cetorhinus maximus) and fin whale (Balaenoptera physalus). Mar Environ Res. 2014 Sep 1;100:17–24. pmid:24612776
  71. 71. Parton KJ, Galloway TS, Godley BJ. Global review of shark and ray entanglement in anthropogenic marine debris. Endanger Species Res. 2019 Jul 4;39:173–90.
  72. 72. Nelms SE, Coombes C, Foster LC, Galloway TS, Godley BJ, Lindeque PK, et al. Marine anthropogenic litter on British beaches: a 10-year nationwide assessment using citizen science data. Sci Total Environ. 2017 Feb 1;579:1399–409. pmid:27913017
  73. 73. Gore MA, Frey PH, Ormond RF, Allan H, Gilkes G. Use of photo-identification and mark-recapture methodology to assess basking shark (Cetorhinus maximus) populations. PLoS One. 2016;11(3). pmid:26930611
  74. 74. Sims DW, Southall EJ, Tarling GA, Metcalfe JD. Habitat‐specific normal and reverse diel vertical migration in the plankton‐feeding basking shark. J Anim Ecol. 2005 Jul;74(4):755–61.
  75. 75. Klimley AP, Beavers SC, Curtis TH, Jorgensen SJ. Movements and swimming behavior of three species of sharks in La Jolla Canyon, California. Environ Biol Fishes. 2002 Feb 1;63(2):117–35.
  76. 76. Nakamura I, Watanabe YY, Papastamatiou YP, Sato K, Meyer CG. Yo-yo vertical movements suggest a foraging strategy for tiger sharks Galeocerdo cuvier. Mar Ecol Prog Ser. 2011 Mar 1;424:237–46.
  77. 77. Iosilevskii G, Papastamatiou YP, Meyer CG, Holland KN. Energetics of the yo-yo dives of predatory sharks. J Theor Biol. 2012 Feb 7;294:172–81. pmid:22108240
  78. 78. Papastamatiou YP, Iosilevskii G, Leos-Barajas V, Brooks EJ, Howey LA, Chapman DD, et al. Optimal swimming strategies and behavioral plasticity of oceanic whitetip sharks. Sci Rep. 2018 Jan 11;8(1):1–2. pmid:29311619
  79. 79. Sims DW, Southall EJ, Humphries NE, Hays GC, Bradshaw CJ, Pitchford JW, et al. Scaling laws of marine predator search behaviour. Nature. 2008 Feb;451(7182):1098–102. pmid:18305542
  80. 80. Jacoby DM, Papastamatiou YP, Freeman R. Inferring animal social networks and leadership: applications for passive monitoring arrays. J R Soc Interface. 2016 Nov 30;13(124):20160676. pmid:27881803
  81. 81. Wing SR, Jack L, Shatova O, Leichter JJ, Barr D, Frew RD, et al. Seabirds and marine mammals redistribute bioavailable iron in the Southern Ocean. Mar Ecol Prog Ser. 2014 Sep 9;510:1–3.
  82. 82. Ratnarajah L, Nicol S, Bowie AR. Pelagic iron recycling in the southern ocean: exploring the contribution of marine animals. Front Mar Sci. 2018 Mar 29;5:109.
  83. 83. Tavares DC, Moura JF, Acevedo-Trejos E, Merico A. Traits shared by marine megafauna and their relationships with ecosystem functions and services. Front Mar Sci. 2019;6:262.
  84. 84. Hettler WF. Influence of temperature and salinity on routine metabolic rate and growth of young Atlantic menhaden. J Fish Biol. 1976 Jan;8(1):55–65.
  85. 85. James AG, Probyn T. The relationship between respiration rate, swimming speed and feeding behaviour in the cape anchovy Engraulis capensis gilchris. J Exp Mar Biol Ecol. 1989 Oct 24;131(2):81–100.
  86. 86. Sims DW. Threshold foraging behaviour of basking sharks on zooplankton: life on an energetic knife-edge?. Proc R Soc Lond [Biol]. 1999 Jul 22;266(1427):1437–43.
  87. 87. Sims DW, Southall EJ, Quayle VA, Fox AM. Annual social behaviour of basking sharks associated with coastal front areas. Proc R Soc Lond [Biol]. 2000 Sep 22;267(1455):1897–904. pmid:11052542
  88. 88. Wilson SG. Basking sharks (Cetorhinus maximus) schooling in the southern Gulf of Maine. Fish Oceanogr. 2004 Jul;13(4):283–6.
  89. 89. Crowe LM, O’brien O, Curtis TH, Leiter SM, Kenney RD, Duley P, et al. Characterization of large basking shark Cetorhinus maximus aggregations in the western North Atlantic Ocean. J Fish Biol. 2018 May;92(5):1371–84. pmid:29516502
  90. 90. Demski LS. Neuroendocrine mechanism controlling the sexual development and behavior of sharks and rays. J Aquacult and Aquat Sci. 1990;5(3):53–67. pmid:20950618
  91. 91. Maricato G, Lodi LF, Abras DR. Assessment of the aerial behaviors of humpback whales, Megaptera novaeangliae (Borowski, 1781), in coastal areas in Northeastern Brazil. Rev Bras Zoo. 2017 Sep 10;18(3).
  92. 92. De Carli RC, Silva FJ, Silva JM. Daily patterns in aerial activity by spinner dolphins in Fernando de Noronha. J Mar Biol Assoc UK. 2018 Aug;98(5):1151–7.
  93. 93. Halsey LG, Iosilevskii G. The energetics of ‘airtime’: estimating swim power from breaching behaviour in fishes and cetaceans. J Exp Biol. 2020 Jan 1;223(1). pmid:31767731
  94. 94. Kavanagh AS, Owen K, Williamson MJ, Blomberg SP, Noad MJ, Goldizen AW, et al. Evidence for the functions of surface‐active behaviors in humpback whales (Megaptera novaeangliae). Mar Mamm Sci. 2017 Jan;33(1):313–34.
  95. 95. Félix F, Botero-Acosta N. Evaluating Humpback Whale (Megaptera novaeangliae) Social Behaviour Through Sexing Active Individuals. Aquat Mamm. 2012 Jul 1;38(3):311.
  96. 96. Martin RA, Hammerschlag N, Collier RS, Fallows C. Predatory behaviour of white sharks (Carcharodon carcharias) at Seal Island, South Africa. J Mar Biol Assoc UK. 2005 Oct 1;85(5):1121–36.
  97. 97. Kuczaj SA, Eskelinen HC. Why do dolphins play. Anim Behav Cogn. 2014;1(2):113–27.
  98. 98. Segre PS, Potvin J, Cade DE, Calambokidis J, Di Clemente J, Fish FE, et al. Energetic and physical limitations on the breaching performance of large whales. Elife. 2020 Mar 11;9:e51760. pmid:32159511
  99. 99. Kotiaho JS. Costs of sexual traits: a mismatch between theoretical considerations and empirical evidence. Biol Rev. 2001 Aug;76(3):365–76. pmid:11569789
  100. 100. Hooker SK, Heaslip SG, Matthiopoulos J, Cox O, Boyd IL. Data sampling options for animal-borne video cameras: considerations based on deployments with Antarctic fur seals. Mar Technol Soc J. 2008 Jun 1;42(2):65–75.
  101. 101. Heaslip SG, Hooker SK. Effect of animal-borne camera and flash on the diving behaviour of the female Antarctic fur seal (Arctocephalus gazella). Deep Sea Res Part I: Oceanogr Res Pap. 2008 Sep 1;55(9):1179–92.