Skip to main content
Advertisement
Browse Subject Areas
?

Click through the PLOS taxonomy to find articles in your field.

For more information about PLOS Subject Areas, click here.

  • Loading metrics

A common variant of CNTNAP2 is associated with sub-threshold autistic traits and intellectual disability

  • Yuka Shiota,

    Roles Conceptualization, Methodology, Project administration, Software, Writing – original draft

    Affiliations United Graduate School of Child Development, Osaka University, Kanazawa University, Hamamatsu University School of Medicine, Chiba University and University of Fukui, Kanazawa, Japan, Research Center for Child Mental Development, Kanazawa University, Kanazawa, Japan

  • Tetsu Hirosawa ,

    Roles Data curation, Funding acquisition, Writing – review & editing

    hirosawatetsu1982@yahoo.co.jp

    Affiliations United Graduate School of Child Development, Osaka University, Kanazawa University, Hamamatsu University School of Medicine, Chiba University and University of Fukui, Kanazawa, Japan, Research Center for Child Mental Development, Kanazawa University, Kanazawa, Japan, Department of Psychiatry and Neurobiology, Graduate School of Medical Science, Kanazawa University, Kanazawa, Japan

  • Yuko Yoshimura,

    Roles Investigation, Resources

    Affiliations United Graduate School of Child Development, Osaka University, Kanazawa University, Hamamatsu University School of Medicine, Chiba University and University of Fukui, Kanazawa, Japan, Institute of Human and Social Sciences, Kanazawa University, Kanazawa, Japan

  • Sanae Tanaka,

    Roles Investigation

    Affiliations United Graduate School of Child Development, Osaka University, Kanazawa University, Hamamatsu University School of Medicine, Chiba University and University of Fukui, Kanazawa, Japan, Research Center for Child Mental Development, Kanazawa University, Kanazawa, Japan

  • Chiaki Hasegawa,

    Roles Investigation, Resources

    Affiliation Research Center for Child Mental Development, Kanazawa University, Kanazawa, Japan

  • Sumie Iwasaki,

    Roles Resources

    Affiliation Research Center for Child Mental Development, Kanazawa University, Kanazawa, Japan

  • Kyung-min An,

    Roles Resources

    Affiliations United Graduate School of Child Development, Osaka University, Kanazawa University, Hamamatsu University School of Medicine, Chiba University and University of Fukui, Kanazawa, Japan, Research Center for Child Mental Development, Kanazawa University, Kanazawa, Japan

  • Daiki Soma,

    Roles Formal analysis

    Affiliation Department of Psychiatry and Neurobiology, Graduate School of Medical Science, Kanazawa University, Kanazawa, Japan

  • Masuhiko Sano,

    Roles Data curation

    Affiliation Department of Psychiatry and Neurobiology, Graduate School of Medical Science, Kanazawa University, Kanazawa, Japan

  • Shigeru Yokoyama,

    Roles Project administration, Resources, Supervision, Writing – review & editing

    Affiliations United Graduate School of Child Development, Osaka University, Kanazawa University, Hamamatsu University School of Medicine, Chiba University and University of Fukui, Kanazawa, Japan, Research Center for Child Mental Development, Kanazawa University, Kanazawa, Japan

  • Mitsuru Kikuchi

    Roles Funding acquisition, Supervision, Writing – review & editing

    Affiliations United Graduate School of Child Development, Osaka University, Kanazawa University, Hamamatsu University School of Medicine, Chiba University and University of Fukui, Kanazawa, Japan, Research Center for Child Mental Development, Kanazawa University, Kanazawa, Japan, Department of Psychiatry and Neurobiology, Graduate School of Medical Science, Kanazawa University, Kanazawa, Japan

Abstract

Sub-threshold autistic traits are common in the general population. Children with sub-threshold autistic traits have difficulties with social adaptation. Contactin-associated protein-like 2 (CNTNAP2) is associated with the development of Autism spectrum disorder (ASD) and the single-nucleotide polymorphism rs2710102 (G/A) of CNTNAP2 is suggested to contribute to sub-threshold social impairments and intellectual disabilities. We recruited 67 children with Autistic disorder (AD) (49 boys, 18 girls, aged 38–98 months) and 57 typically developing (TD) children (34 boys, 23 girls, aged 53–90 months). We assessed the participants’ intelligence and social reciprocity using the Kaufman Assessment Battery for Children (K-ABC) and the Social Responsiveness Scale (SRS), respectively. Genomic DNA was extracted from the buccal mucosa and genotyped for rs2710102. A chi-square test revealed a significant association between genotype and group [χ2(2) = 6.56, p = 0.038]. When a co-dominant model was assumed, the results from linear regression models demonstrated that TD children with A-carriers (AA + AG) presented higher SRS T-scores [t(55) = 2.11, p = 0.039] and lower simultaneous processing scale scores of K-ABC [t(55) = -2.19, p = 0.032] than those with GG homozygotes. These associations were not significant in children with ASD. TD children with the rs2710102 A-allele may have more sub-threshold autistic traits than those with GG homozygotes, reflected in higher SRS scores and lower simultaneous processing scale scores. These results support the use of genetic evidence to detect sub-threshold autistic traits.

Introduction

Autism spectrum disorder (ASD) is a neurodevelopmental disorder characterized by high heritability and deficits in social communication, restricted interests, and repetitive patterns of behavior [1]. Although the prevalence of ASD is estimated at only 1.85% [2], autistic social deficiency is common in the general population [3]. Given its continuous distribution, the boundary between typically developing (TD) children and children affected by ASD is somewhat ambiguous. In this context, children with less prominent autistic traits than those with fully diagnosed ASD are referred to as having sub-threshold autism traits [4]. Consequently, children with sub-threshold autistic traits tend to have difficulties in social adaptation, albeit they are not diagnosed with full-fledged ASD. The increasing evidence suggests that difficulties in social adaptation may result in an increased risk of comorbid mental health problems. For example, individuals with sub-threshold autistic traits have a higher prevalence of anxiety, depression, hyperactivity, problematic behavior, and more prone to substance abuse [5, 6]. Furthermore, sub-threshold autistic traits at age five predict later emotional symptoms and peer problems at age seven in the general population [7]. Overall, combined evidence from the literature suggest that early detection and appropriate support are necessary not only for children with ASD, but for those with sub-threshold autistic traits.

In line with the continuous distribution of autistic traits, almost all genetic risk factors for ASD can also be observed in the general population. Earlier studies have suggested associations between genetic risk factors and social impairments in children with ASD and the general population [810]. For example, Robinson et al. investigated several large ASD in consortia and the general population (total n > 38,000) and reported that multiple types of genetic risk for ASD influence a continuum of autistic traits, the severe tail of which can result in a diagnosis of ASD or other neuropsychiatric disorders [8]. From this perspective, although it is difficult to fully elucidate the genetic etiology of ASD considering its heterogeneous nature, utilizing evidence from genetic studies may be a promising approach to detect sub-threshold autistic traits.

Many genes, including common and rare variants, have been identified as the basis for ASD [1113]. Among these, association, linkage, gene expression, and imaging studies suggest the role of both common and rare variants of contactin-associated protein-like 2 (CNTNAP2). Originally, a recessive nonsense mutation in CNTNAP2 was observed to cause cortical dysplasia focal epilepsy syndrome, a syndromic form of ASD [14]. Several reports have linked this gene with an increased risk of autism [15, 16]. Among single nucleotide polymorphisms (SNPs) of CNTNAP2, rs2710102 (G/A, intron13) is one of the most frequently reported to be associated with autistic traits [17]. G-allele of rs2710102 has been associated with language impairments in individuals with and without ASD [1820], but more importantly, having an A-allele of this SNP is associated with autistic traits in the general population [2123], for example, having high AQ-social scores [21] and high social anxiety-related traits [22] in young adults (i.e., college students). Steer et al. reported that having the A-allele in children is associated with difficulties in inhibition in the context of social communication (i.e., social inhibition), albeit they did not directly investigate the relation between rs2710102 and autistic traits [23]. Although the association between rs2710102 and social and language impairment has been repeatedly demonstrated, Werling et al. meta-analyzed the association between rs2710102 and the diagnosis of ASD or high functioning ASD and reported that this association was non-significant [24]. Consequently, rs2710102 seems to confer sub-threshold autistic traits, the effect of which is sufficient to result in full-fledged ASD. This hypothesis encouraged us to utilize rs2710102 to detect individuals with sub-threshold autistic traits. Earlier detection and support have great merit in this context, however no previous study has directly focused on the association between rs2710102 and autistic traits during childhood. Furthermore, no studies have been conducted on the effect of rs2710102 on autistic symptoms in individuals with ASD. Therefore, the association between the A-allele and more prominent autistic symptoms remains unexplored in this population.

In addition to its association with autistic traits, biallelic defect in CNTNAP2 has been reported to cause fully penetrant, severe forms of broad-spectrum neurodevelopmental disorders, such as cortical dysplasia and focal epilepsy syndrome [14, 25], and intellectual disability and epilepsy resembling Pitt-Hopkins syndrome [26]. Supporting the association between CNTNAP2 and intellectual disability, Gregor et al. reported that even heterozygous variants or defects in CNTNAP2 are associated with moderate to the severe intellectual disability with or without epilepsy [27]. Considering the association between CNTNAP2 and intellectual disabilities, one might infer that rs2710102 itself is related to intelligence. Supporting this hypothesis, Uddin et al. recently conducted a case-control study followed by a meta-analysis and concluded that the presence of the G-allele of rs2710102 is associated with language impairment in children with and without ASD [28]. Similarly, the G-allele was associated with lower language skills in individuals with specific language impairment [12]. However, this putative association between rs2710102 and intelligence has not been directly examined. Notably, evidence suggests the complex relation among rs2710102, autistic traits, and language impairment. G-allele of rs2710102 is reported to be the risk factor for language impairment; however, A-allele is reported to be the risk factor for autistic traits.

In this study, we aimed to determine if and how rs2710102 is associated with sub-threshold autistic traits or intellectual disability in children with and without ASD. Considering the previous studies, we hypothesized that (i) carriers of the A-allele would have higher autistic traits compared to GG homozygotes in both populations and that (ii) carriers of the A-allele would also have higher intelligence compared to GG homozygotes in both populations.

Materials and methods

Participants

This study included 77 Japanese children with Autistic disorder (AD) and 57 TD children (91 boys and 43 girls, aged 38–98 months). Participants were recruited from Kanazawa University and affiliated hospitals. We confirmed that no TD child had a history of neurological, neuropsychiatric, or neurodevelopmental disorders. The diagnosis of AD was established according to the Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders (4th edition) [29] using the Diagnostic Interview for Social and Communication Disorders [30] or the Autism Diagnostic Observation Schedule-Generic [31] or the Autism Diagnostic Observation Schedule 2 [32], details of which are given in the S1 and S2 Tables. We excluded participants with blindness, deafness, and other neuropsychiatric disorders, including epilepsy and ongoing medication. Intelligence is also an important factor in ASD, and high- and low-functioning autism are often distinguished based on intelligence quotient (IQ) scores [33]. “High functioning autism” is a term used for individuals with ASD who have an intelligence of 70 or above, especially indicating individuals without moderate to severe intellectual disability. ASDs with lower intelligence and those with higher intelligence are potentially different in terms of their cognitive profiles and genetic etiology. For example, Szatmari et al. (1998) reported that different genetic factors may account for the higher and lower functioning forms of autism [34]. Thus, we excluded 10 participants with an IQ <70. IQ was assessed using the mental processing composite scale of the Kaufman Assessment Battery for Children (K-ABC). Overall, we analyzed 67 children with AD (49 boys, 18 girls, aged 38–98 months) and 57 TD children (34 boys, 23 girls, aged 53–90 months). These are also part of an ongoing project (Bambi plan, http://bambiplan.w3.kanazawa-u.ac.jp/pdf/jusen_english.pdf). We are continually recruiting participants as part of a large epidemiological study, and some participants overlapped with those in our earlier study [35]. However, their results did not overlap. Additionally, the emphases of the earlier study differed from those of this study. The study was approved by the institutional review board of Kanazawa University Hospital and conducted per tenets of the Declaration of Helsinki [36]. Written informed consent was obtained from all participants’ guardians.

Genotyping

Buccal mucosa cells were obtained from all participants by gently scraping the mucosa with a cotton swab. DNA was extracted from the cells, followed by whole genome amplification using the PicoPLEX WGA kit (Takara Bio, Mountain View, CA, USA). DNA concentrations were measured using a Qubit4 fluorometer (Thermo Fisher Scientific, Waltham, MA, USA). Genotyping of rs2710102 was performed by real-time PCR (ViiA 7, Applied Biosystems, Foster City, CA, USA) using the rhAmp SNP Genotyping system (assay name: CD.GT.TKKG0518.1: Integrated DNA Technologies, Coralville, IA, USA). The following RNA-DNA hybrid primers were used: 5′-TTGGTTAACATTTACTCTGAGACC[T/C]GAGAA-3′, and 5′-GCTGGAGTGAACCTGTTTGATTATTGCTGAT-3′ as locus-specific primers (https://sg.idtdna.com/pages/products/qpcr-and-pcr/genotyping/rhamp-snp-genotyping).

Assessment of social reciprocity

The Social Responsiveness Scale (SRS) [37] was used to assess the participants’ social reciprocity: A 65-item rating scale that measures sociality and autistic mannerisms as a quantitative trait in TD children and children with ASD [3840]. It measures subscales of social awareness, social cognition, social communication, social motivation, and autistic mannerisms to generate a single measure. We used gender-normed social responsiveness scale T scores (SRS-T) of each subscale [41]. Higher scores indicated greater difficulties in social reciprocity. The SRS has been one of the most frequently used quantitative measures of ASD symptoms, with very strong measurement properties in healthy volunteers and clinical cases of ASD [42]. In this context, this questionnaire is suitable for measuring sub-threshold autistic traits.

The participants’ parents filled out this questionnaire indicating how strongly they agreed with each question by checking one of the following: “never true,” “sometimes true,” “often true,” or “always true,” that were further scored as one, two, three, and four points, respectively [43]. Consequently, T-scores of SRS reflect the children’s social reciprocity in their natural social contexts [37]. It can describe moderate deficiencies in reciprocal social behavior that are “clinically significant” in ASD as well as in the general population [3].

Assessment of intelligence

The Japanese version of the K-ABC [44], based on neuropsychological and information-processing theories, was used to assess the intelligence of all participants, with a distinction between problem-solving and knowledge of facts. The former was interpreted as intelligence, whereas the latter was defined as an achievement. Problem-solving abilities were measured using two mental processing scales [45], the sequential processing scale, which requires the child to solve problems in serial or temporal order, and the simultaneous processing scale, which requires the child to integrate multiple stimuli simultaneously to solve problems. The mental processing composite scale is a unification of the sequential and simultaneous processing scales, intended to measure total general intelligence. The achievement scale measures the degree of knowledge and skills that children acquire from the environment. These scores are provided as age-adjusted standardized scores normalized to a mean of 100 and a standard deviation of 15.

One of the goals of developing K-ABC was to separate intelligence from other factors, including achievement. Notably, although the achievement scale was reported to be well-correlated with language skills, the association of language skills with the two mental processing scales was reported to be weak. For example, Wiebe et al. reported that the correlation coefficient between the achievement scale and verbal IQ measured by Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children-Revised was 0.77. In the same study, the correlation coefficient between sequential processing scale or simultaneous processing and verbal IQ were 0.54 and 0.63, respectively [46]. Since our objective was to examine the effect of rs2710102 on intelligence rather than on language skills, we focused on two mental processing scales in this study.

Statistical analysis

Statistical analyses were performed using Stata software (ver. 16.1; Stata Corp., College Station, TX, USA). First, we checked the differences in age and scores in SRS-T and K-ABC between children with AD and TD using Student’s t-tests. Sex differences were tested using the chi-square test. Further, we employed a chi-square test to compare the group (AD or TD) and rs2710102 genotypes. Based on these results, we assumed a proper genetic model and evaluated the risk allele for AD. Based on the genetic model, we investigated the association between social reciprocity and the participants’ genotypic status after controlling for the disease group. In particular, we applied linear regression models to predict SRS total T-scores based on the participants’ group, genotype, and the interaction between group and genotype. If a significant interaction was observed or when appropriate, we investigated the association between genetic status and SRS-T score in each group. In addition to SRS total T-scores, as an exploratory analysis, we examined the correlation between each subscale of SRS and the genotype. In those models, we used each subscale of SRS (i.e., social awareness subscale, social cognitive subscale, social communication subscale, social motivation subscale, and autistic mannerisms sub-scale) as dependent variables instead of total SRS-T scores.

We also applied linear regression models to predict the K-ABC two mental processing scales (simultaneous processing scale and sequential processing scale) based on the participants’ group, genotype, and interaction with the group and adopted the same model for the achievement scale. A p-value <0.05 indicated statistical significance. If a significant interaction effect was observed or when appropriate, we applied post-hoc analysis to elucidate the association between the genotype and K-ABC scores in each group. In the post-hoc analyses, we predicted the sequential processing scale or simultaneous processing scale based on the participants’ group, genotype, and interaction with the group.

As an exploratory analysis, to control possible effects of SRS total T-scores on the scores in K-ABC or vice-versa, we analyzed the effect of genotype on SRS total T-scores controlling on the effect of intelligence. We further examined the effect of genotype on intelligence controlling for the effect of SRS total T-scores. Specifically, we applied linear regression models in each participant’s group (i.e., AD or TD) to predict (i) SRS total T-scores based on the genotype and simultaneous processing scale in K-ABC, (ii) SRS total T-scores based on the genotype and sequential processing scale in K-ABC, (iii) simultaneous processing scale in K-ABC based on the genotype and SRS total T-scores, and (iv) sequential processing scale in K-ABC based on the genotype and SRS total T-scores.

Before applying linear regression, we verified that our data met the assumptions for the regression analysis. Specifically, we used standard methods to verify linearity, normality, homogeneity of variance, model specifications, influence, and collinearity. However, the assumption of homogeneity was violated in some regression models. Therefore, we used heteroskedasticity-robust standard errors [47].

Results

Participants’ characteristics

The distribution of CNTNAP2 rs2710102 (A/G) genotypes among the 124 sampled subjects were as follows: Twenty-eight participants were homozygous AA genotype, 67 participants carried the heterozygous AG, and 29 were homozygous GG. Tests for the Hardy-Weinberg equilibrium exhibited no deviations from the expected genotype distribution (p > 0.05). The SRS total T-score was significantly higher in children with AD compared to that in TD children [t(120) = -12.23, p < 0.001]. The mental processing composite scale score was significantly lower in children with AD than in TD children [t(120) = 3.05, p = 0.0027]. This observation was consistent across sequential and simultaneous processing scales. The sequential processing scale and simultaneous processing scale scores were significantly lower in children with AD than in TD children [t(120) = 3.46, p = 0.0007; and t(120) = 2.40, p = 0.0177, respectively]. However, we observed no significant differences in achievement scale scores. The participant characteristics are presented in Table 1. There was no significant difference in age or sex between the groups.

Association between participant groups and genotypes

A chi-square test revealed a significant difference between the genotypes and groups (χ2(2) = 6.56, p = 0.038). Allele and genotype frequencies are presented in Table 2. The genetic model was determined based on these results. According to the selected genetic model [48], we transformed the three genotypes (AA, AG, and GG) into two variables. In particular, considering our results and the findings of previous studies [21], we assumed the co-dominant model: Carriers of the A-allele (AA + AG) versus G-allele homozygotes. This indicates that allele A increases autistic traits. Additionally, we employed a chi-square test to investigate the difference between carriers of the A-allele and the groups. However, this difference was not significant (χ2(2) = 1.29, p = 0.256).

Association between genotypes and SRS total T-scores

We applied linear regression models to predict the SRS total T-score based on the participants’ group (AD or TD), genotype (carriers of the A-allele or G-allele homozygotes), and interaction between group and genotype. Table 3 and Fig 1 summarize results of regression models. In this model, carriers of the A-allele and the group were significantly affected [t(120) = 2.11, p = 0.036] and [t(120) = 8.82, p < 0.001], respectively, details of which are given in the S1 Fig.

thumbnail
Fig 1. Bar charts present the SRS total T-scores between genotypes in children with autistic disorder (AD) and typically developing (TD) children (mean ± SD).

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0260548.g001

Additionally, the F statistic was significant [F(3, 120) = 65.63, p < 0.001]. However, the interaction was not significant [t(120) = -1.24, p = 0.216]. For this particular model, the assumption of normality was violated. According to the histogram, this was caused by different patterns of association in AD or TD. Hence, we applied linear regression to predict the SRS total T-score based on genotypes in each group. As a result, a significant main effect of genotype was observed for TD [t(55) = 2.11, p = 0.039]. However, this association was not significant in children with AD.

As an exploratory analysis, to further investigate the association between the genotype and social reciprocity, we examined the correlation between each subscale of SRS and the genotype. Aside from the main effect of the participants’ group, we observed a significant effect only for the model predicting social awareness subscale. Specifically, we observed a main effect of the genotype to be significant [t(120) = 1.99, p = 0.049] for the model, indicating that carriers of the A-allele had a higher social awareness subscale of SRS. Post-hoc analysis demonstrated that this effect was larger in the TD group, although it did not reach statistical significance [t(55) = 1.98, p = 0.52]. The results are presented in S3 Table.

Association between genotypes on K-ABC scores

For the model predicting the mental processing composite scale based on the participants’ group (AD or TD), genotype (carriers of the A-allele or G-allele homozygotes), and interaction with the group, only the main effect of the group was observed to be significant [t(120) = -2.74, p = 0.007]. No other factors were observed to be statistically significant. For the model predicting the simultaneous processing scale, the main effect of genotype was observed to be significant [t(120) = -2.19, p = 0.030], and the group [t(120) = -3.43, p < 0.001] was also observed to be significant. The interaction effect was also significant [t(120) = 2.55, p = 0.011]. Hence, we applied a post-hoc analysis to predict the simultaneous processing scale based on the genotypes in each group, and a significant main effect of genotype was observed only for TD [t(55) = -2.19, p = 0.033], scatter plot of which are provided in the S2 Fig. Table 4 summarizes the results of the models predicting the simultaneous processing scale. For the model predicting the sequential processing scale, no factor was observed to be significant.

thumbnail
Table 4. Association between genotypes on simultaneous processing scale of K-ABC.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0260548.t004

Association between genotypes and SRS total T-scores controlling for the effect of K-ABC scores, and the association between genotypes and K-ABC scores controlling for the effect of SRS total T-scores.

Results were slightly similar to those presented above. In TD children, carriers of the A-allele had significantly higher SRS total T-scores after controlling for the effect of sequential processing scale [t(54) = 2.10, p = 0.04], however statistical significance was not seen while controlling for the effect of simultaneous processing scale [t(54) = 1.78, p = 0.08]. For the models predicting simultaneous processing scores, carriers of the A-allele had significantly lower simultaneous processing scores after controlling for the effect of SRS total T-scores [t(64) = -2.17, p = 0.03]. In children with AD, no significant effect was observed. The results are presented in S4 Table.

Discussions

To our knowledge, this is the first study to reveal a significant association between common variants of the autism-related gene, rs2710102 in CNTNAP2 and autistic traits and intelligence in TD Japanese children. Particularly, we observed that TD children carrying the A-allele presented significantly higher SRS total T-scores compared to G-allele homozygotes. We also observed that TD children carrying the A-allele had significantly lower simultaneous processing scores compared to G-allele homozygotes. However, these associations were not significant for children with AD.

Our results indicated that carriers of the A-allele have high SRS total T-scores in TD children; however, this relation was non-significant in children with AD. This result was consistent with the previous findings in young adults (i.e., college students), where carriers of the A-allele are associated with social difficulties, including high AQ-social scores [21] and social anxiety-related traits [22]. In this context, our results confirmed the association between the presence of A-allele and autistic traits, extending it to the younger population (i.e., 38–98 months). In a similar age range, Steer et al. examined 13,138 children aged 6 months to 9 years old and reported that the presence of A-allele is associated with difficulties in social inhibition [23], however they did not directly investigate the relation between rs2710102 and autistic traits. Therefore, this study was the first to demonstrate that TD children carrying the A-allele of rs2710102 already have subclinical autistic traits as determined by the SRS, since early childhood. Further studies are thus needed to investigate how such subtle social difficulties affect their functional outcomes in adulthood. From a clinical perspective, this population might benefit from interventions that focus on boosting social-emotional, cognitive, and language abilities (e.g., early start Denver model). Future clinical trials that utilize rs2710102 to detect children with subthreshold autistic traits will be beneficial to confirm the efficacy of such interventions.

The association between rs2710102 and autistic traits, however, was not significant in children with AD. This discrepancy can be explained by the genetic characteristics of AD. AD is with a wide variety of mutations and consequently, autistic symptoms are affected by many genes. In this context, autistic traits might have already been “saturated” in children with full-fledged AD, and thus, one SNP in CNTNAP2 may not be sufficient to significantly alter social reciprocity in this population. Alternatively, the effect of this SNP on autistic traits can be buffered by other autism genes and their molecular pathways. In this case, rs2710102 may exert its effect on interactions with other genes, some of which (possibly autism genes) may mitigate their effects on autistic traits, while others may exacerbate them. Further studies focusing on the relation between rs2710102 and autistic traits in other developmental disorders will be beneficial to clarify whether this polymorphism specifically affects TD children.

In addition to social reciprocity, unlike our hypothesis, we observed significantly lower simultaneous processing scores in carriers of the A-allele in TD children, whereas the effect was non-significant in children with AD. This result was consistent with the previous findings revealing that balletic defects [26] or heterozygous variant or defects in CNTNAP2 are associated with moderate to severe intellectual disability [27]. Additionally, our results provided another evidence supporting the role of CNTNAP2 in intelligence, by presenting that the association may even be caused by a single SNP (i.e., rs2710102). The risk allele was different from that for language impairment. In contrast to our results, the G-allele has been observed to be associated with language impairment in children with AD [28], as well as specific language impairment [19], and in the general population [17]. Our results thus extend those previous findings in two meaningful ways: (i) The effect of this SNP in rs2710102 on brain function is not only limited to language skills in TD children, but also extends to intelligence and autistic traits. In children with AD, however, its effect on intelligence and autistic traits would not be as strong as that in TD children. (ii) The effect of this SNP on brain function is not straightforward. Carriers of the A-allele of rs2710102 may have better language skills, albeit they also have lower intelligence and more prominent autistic traits. Overall, the complex structure of the relation between rs2710102, intelligence, autistic traits and language skills is remarkable. The effect of rs2710102 on brain function and risk allele seem to be different depending on the presence of AD and the domain of the function (i.e., A-allele for intelligence and autistic traits, G-allele for language skills), respectively. These results support the hypothesis that rs2710102 exerts its effect on complex interactions with other genes, such that autism genes may not only affect the autistic traits, but also intelligence.

The sequential and simultaneous cognitive processes have been associated with specific anatomical regions of the brain [4951]. In general, the left hemisphere is responsible for processing language and performing sequential processing of information [52]. Meanwhile, the right hemisphere is considered specialized for simultaneous processing [52]. Consequently, our findings could be attributable to right hemispheric dysfunction in carriers of the A-allele. Although the right hemisphere has been considered the nonverbal side, it helps language processes in the left hemisphere and plays a role in cognitive-communication [53]. In particular, the right hemisphere appears to be critical for pragmatic communication [54]. Pragmatic communication refers to the ability to use language in context. For example, it includes the ability of verbal and non-verbal communication and the ability to use context cues for understanding verbal and non-verbal communication [55]. Notably, impairment of these abilities is an essential feature of AD. Indeed, high-functioning autism has been linked with a communication disorder known as a semantic-pragmatic disorder, speculated as a dysfunction of the right hemisphere [5658]. From this perspective, the observed subclinical autistic traits in TD children carrying the A-allele could also be attributable to right hemispheric dysfunction, which results in impaired pragmatic communication.

The association between CNTNAP2 and the right hemisphere further supports this view. For example, G-allele homozygotes of rs2710102 reportedly have significantly increased activation of the right inferior frontal gyrus during a language task in healthy adults compared to carriers of the A-allele [20]. Carriers of the G-allele demonstrate greater right frontal connectivity compared to non-G-carriers [59]. Furthermore, evidence suggests that even a single SNP in CNTNAP2 may be sufficient to cause a significant change in right hemisphere function. For example, Riva et al. reported an association of the rs2710102 G/G genotype with lower P3 amplitude in the right hemisphere during rapid auditory processing, which in turn predicted later poor expressive vocabulary [18]. Koeda et al. reported that A/T heterozygotes in rs7794745, another SNP of CNTNAP2, presented a reduction in the right frontal activity of language processing compared to A/A homozygotes in healthy adults [60]. Combining all the evidence, based on the presumable association between rs2710102 of CNTNAP2 and the right hemisphere, it can be hypothesized that in TD individuals, CNTNAP2 polymorphism affects functions of the right hemisphere. Therefore, carriers of the A-allele of rs2710102 would affect the functions of the right hemisphere, causing lower simultaneous processing scores.

Limitations

This study has several limitations. First, the sample size was too small to investigate behavioral characteristics. Larger samples are thus needed to clarify characteristics of autistic traits in TD children. Second, we did not use multiple tests in the current study. Most genetic studies with relatively few polymorphisms are not sufficiently powered to derive definitive conclusions when adjusting the results for multiple testing [61]. Further, adjusting for multiple tests may increase the chance of type II errors owing to the conservative nature of the Bonferroni adjustment [62]. Third, we need to confirm the association between these results and neural substrates. The ongoing study may assist in providing more evidence on genetic-behavior-brain interactions using neuroimaging methods. Fourth, we could not prove a cause-and-effect association between the genetic changes and behavioral characteristics. Further studies are thus required to elucidate the underlying biological mechanisms. Moreover, all our participants were children, and we did not include any adolescents or adults. These results should not be generalized unless validated in older individuals. Fifth, although the K-ABC second edition (K-ABC II, [63]) was released in 2004, we used the original version of K-ABC [64]. Similarly, we used the original version of SRS rather than SRS-2 [41], which is available from 2017. This was because the Japanese version of neither the K-ABC second edition nor SRS second edition had been verified at the time we started this study. However, in any case, this did not align with the current best practice standards. Sixth, all the TD children were native Japanese with no previous or existing developmental, learning, or behavioral problems according to information obtained from questionnaires completed by their parents. Consequently, it might be possible that some children we classified as TD had AD. This should be especially considered for those who had higher SRS total T-scores.

Conclusions

Our findings suggest that genetic variation in rs2710102 of CNTNAP2 is associated with sub-threshold autistic traits and intelligence in TD children. One issue of practical interest is the possible use of evidence from genetic studies to detect sub-threshold autistic traits. This application is attractive in clinical practice, however it still needs to overcome reasonable limitations (e.g., determining who requires appropriate support or the extent to which individuals are in need). Additional studies need to be conducted in this respect.

Supporting information

S1 Fig. Scatter plots of the distribution of the variance in SRS total T-scores.

Scatter plots present the SRS total T-scores for carriers of the A-allele and GG genotype among children with autistic disorder (AD) and typically developing (TD) children. Some children in the AD group had lower (<60) SRS total T-scores, albeit their diagnosis of AD was confirmed using ADOS or DISCO, both of which are the golden standards for diagnosing ASD.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0260548.s001

(TIFF)

S2 Fig. Scatter plots of the distribution in the simultaneous processing scale.

Scatter plots present the simultaneous processing scale for carriers of the A-allele and GG genotype among children with autistic disorder (AD) and typically developing (TD) children.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0260548.s002

(TIFF)

S1 Table. Scores and sub-scores of ADOS in children with autistic disorder.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0260548.s003

(DOCX)

S2 Table. Scores and sub-scores of ADOS-2 in children with autistic disorder.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0260548.s004

(DOCX)

S3 Table. Association between genotypes and sub-scales of SRS.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0260548.s005

(DOCX)

S4 Table. Association between genotypes and SRS total T-scores controlling for the effect of K-ABC scores, and association between genotypes and K-ABC scores controlling for the effect of SRS total T-scores.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0260548.s006

(DOCX)

Acknowledgments

We are grateful to all study participants and our colleagues at the Bambi Plan for their support. We would like to thank Editage (www.editage.com) for English language editing.

References

  1. 1. American Psychiatric Association. Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders. 5th ed. 2013.
  2. 2. Maenner MJ, Shaw KA, Baio J, Washington A, Patrick M, et al. Prevalence of Autism Spectrum Disorder Among Children Aged 8 Years—Autism and Developmental Disabilities Monitoring Network, 11 Sites, United States, 2016. MMWR Surveill Summ. 2020; 69: 1–12. pmid:32214087
  3. 3. Constantino JN, Todd RD. Autistic traits in the general population: a twin study. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 2003; 60: 524–530. pmid:12742874
  4. 4. Gökçen E, Petrides KV, Hudry K, Frederickson N, Smillie LD. Sub-threshold autism traits: the role of trait emotional intelligence and cognitive flexibility. Br J Psychol. 2014; 105: 187–199. pmid:24754807
  5. 5. Christ SE, Kanne SM, Reiersen AM. Executive function in individuals with subthreshold autism traits. Neuropsychology. 2010; 24: 590–598. pmid:20804247
  6. 6. Lundström S, Chang Z, Kerekes N, Gumpert CH, Råstam M, Gillberg C, et al. Autistic-like traits and their association with mental health problems in two nationwide twin cohorts of children and adults. Psychol Med. 2011; 41: 2423–2433. pmid:21426604
  7. 7. Saito A, Stickley A, Haraguchi H, Takahashi H, Ishitobi M, Kamio Y. Association Between Autistic Traits in Preschool Children and Later Emotional/Behavioral Outcomes. J Autism Dev Disord. 2017; 47: 3333–3346. pmid:28785972
  8. 8. Robinson EB, St Pourcain B, Anttila V, Kosmicki JA, Bulik-Sullivan B, Grove J, et al. Genetic risk for autism spectrum disorders and neuropsychiatric variation in the general population. Nat Genet. 2016; 48: 552–555. pmid:26998691
  9. 9. Bralten J, van Hulzen KJ, Martens MB, Galesloot TE, Arias Vasquez A, Kiemeney LA, et al. Autism spectrum disorders and autistic traits share genetics and biology. Mol Psychiatry. 2018; 23: 1205–1212. pmid:28507316
  10. 10. Lundström S, Chang Z, Råstam M, Gillberg C, Larsson H, Anckarsäter H, et al. Autism spectrum disorders and autistic like traits: similar etiology in the extreme end and the normal variation. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 2012; 69: 46–52. pmid:22213788
  11. 11. Sebat J, Lakshmi B, Malhotra D, Troge J, Lese-Martin C, Walsh T, et al. Strong association of de novo copy number mutations with autism. Science. 2007; 316: 445–449. pmid:17363630
  12. 12. Glessner JT, Wang K, Cai G, Korvatska O, Kim CE, Wood S, et al. Autism genome-wide copy number variation reveals ubiquitin and neuronal genes. Nature. 2009; 459: 569–573. pmid:19404257
  13. 13. Weiss LA, Arking DE, Gene Discovery Project of Johns Hopkins & the Autism Consortium, Daly MJ, Chakravarti A. A genome-wide linkage and association scan reveal novel loci for autism. Nature. 2009; 461: 802–808. pmid:19812673
  14. 14. Strauss KA, Puffenberger EG, Huentelman MJ, Gottlieb S, Dobrin SE, Parod JM, et al. Recessive symptomatic focal epilepsy and mutant contactin-associated protein-like 2. N Engl J Med. 2006; 354: 1370–1377. pmid:16571880
  15. 15. Arking DE, Cutler DJ, Brune CW, Teslovich TM, West K, Ikeda M, et al. A common genetic variant in the neurexin superfamily member CNTNAP2 increases familial risk of autism. Am J Hum Genet. 2008; 82: 160–164. pmid:18179894
  16. 16. Bakkaloglu B, O’Roak BJ, Louvi A, Gupta AR, Abelson JF, Morgan TM, et al. Molecular cytogenetic analysis and resequencing of contactin associated protein-like 2 in autism spectrum disorders. Am J Hum Genet. 2008; 82: 165–173. pmid:18179895
  17. 17. Whitehouse AJ, Bishop DV, Ang QW, Pennell CE, Fisher SE. CNTNAP2 variants affect early language development in the general population. Genes Brain Behav. 2011; 10: 451–456. pmid:21310003
  18. 18. Riva V, Cantiani C, Benasich AA, Molteni M, Piazza C, Giorda R, et al. From CNTNAP2 to Early Expressive Language in Infancy: The Mediation Role of Rapid Auditory Processing. Cereb Cortex. 2018; 28: 2100–2108. pmid:28498932
  19. 19. Vernes SC, Newbury DF, Abrahams BS, Winchester L, Nicod J, Groszer M, et al. A functional genetic link between distinct developmental language disorders. N Engl J Med. 2008; 359: 2337–2345. pmid:18987363
  20. 20. Whalley HC, O’Connell G, Sussmann JE, Peel A, Stanfield AC, Hayiou-Thomas ME, et al. Genetic variation in CNTNAP2 alters brain function during linguistic processing in healthy individuals. Am J Med Genet B Neuropsychiatr Genet. 2011; 156B: 941–948. pmid:21987501
  21. 21. Bai T, Zhang L, Xie X, Xiao G, Huang W, Li D, et al. Common variant of CNTNAP2 gene modulates the social performances and functional connectivity of posterior right temporoparietal junction. Soc Cogn Affect Neurosci. 2019; 14: 1297–1305. pmid:31993662
  22. 22. Stein MB, Yang BZ, Chavira DA, Hitchcock CA, Sung SC, Shipon-Blum E, et al. A common genetic variant in the neurexin superfamily member CNTNAP2 is associated with increased risk for selective mutism and social anxiety-related traits. Biol Psychiatry. 2011; 69: 825–831. pmid:21193173
  23. 23. Steer CD, Golding J, Bolton PF. Traits contributing to the autistic spectrum. PLOS ONE. 2010; 5: e12633. pmid:20838614
  24. 24. Werling AM, Bobrowski E, Taurines R, Gundelfinger R, Romanos M, Grünblatt E, et al. CNTNAP2 gene in high functioning autism: no association according to family and meta-analysis approaches. J Neural Transm (Vienna). 2016; 123: 353–363. pmid:26559825
  25. 25. Jackman C, Horn ND, Molleston JP, Sokol DK. Gene associated with seizures, autism, and hepatomegaly in an Amish girl. Pediatr Neurol. 2009; 40: 310–313. pmid:19302947
  26. 26. Zweier C, de Jong EK, Zweier M, Orrico A, Ousager LB, Collins AL, et al. CNTNAP2 and NRXN1 are mutated in autosomal-recessive Pitt-Hopkins-like mental retardation and determine the level of a common synaptic protein in Drosophila. Am J Hum Genet. 2009; 85: 655–666. pmid:19896112
  27. 27. Gregor A, Albrecht B, Bader I, Bijlsma EK, Ekici AB, Engels H, et al. Expanding the clinical spectrum associated with defects in CNTNAP2 and NRXN1. BMC Med Genet. 2011; 12: 106. pmid:21827697
  28. 28. Uddin MS, Azima A, Aziz MA, Aka TD, Jafrin S, Millat MS, et al. CNTNAP2 gene polymorphisms in autism spectrum disorder and language impairment among Bangladeshi children: a case-control study combined with a meta-analysis. Hum Cell. 2021; 34: 1410–1423. pmid:33950402
  29. 29. Association AP. Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders: DSM-IV-TR. 4th ed 2000.
  30. 30. Wing L, Leekam SR, Libby SJ, Gould J, Larcombe M. The Diagnostic Interview for Social and Communication Disorders: background, inter-rater reliability and clinical use. J Child Psychol Psychiatry. 2002; 43: 307–325. pmid:11944874
  31. 31. Lord C, Risi S, Lambrecht L, Cook EH Jr., Leventhal BL, DiLavore PC, et al. The autism diagnostic observation schedule-generic: a standard measure of social and communication deficits associated with the spectrum of autism. J Autism Dev Disord. 2000; 30: 205–223. pmid:11055457
  32. 32. Lord C, Rutter M, Luyster RJ, Gotham K. Autism Diagnostic Observation Schedule, 2nd Ed.
  33. 33. Volkmar F, Chawarska K, Klin A. Autism in infancy and early childhood. Annu Rev Psychol. 2005; 56: 315–336. pmid:15709938
  34. 34. Szatmari P, Jones MB, Zwaigenbaum L, MacLean JE. Genetics of autism: overview and new directions. J Autism Dev Disord. 1998; 28: 351–368. pmid:9813773
  35. 35. Hirosawa T, Kontani K, Fukai M, Kameya M, Soma D, Hino S, et al. Different associations between intelligence and social cognition in children with and without autism spectrum disorders. PLOS ONE. 2020; 15: e0235380. pmid:32822358
  36. 36. World Medical Association. World Medical Association Declaration of Helsinki. Law Med Health Care. 1991; 264–5. pmid:11642954
  37. 37. Constantino JN, Gruber CP. Social responsiveness scale [Manual]. Los Angeles. CA: Western Psychological Services; 2005.
  38. 38. Constantino JN, Gruber CP, Davis S, Hayes S, Passanante N, Przybeck T. The factor structure of autistic traits. J Child Psychol Psychiatry. 2004; 45: 719–726. pmid:15056304
  39. 39. Kamio Y, Inada N, Moriwaki A, Kuroda M, Koyama T, Tsujii H, et al. Quantitative autistic traits ascertained in a national survey of 22 529 Japanese schoolchildren. Acta Psychiatr Scand. 2013; 128: 45–53. pmid:23171198
  40. 40. Bölte S, Poustka F, Constantino JN. Assessing autistic traits: cross-cultural validation of the social responsiveness scale (SRS). Autism Res. 2008; 1: 354–363. pmid:19360690
  41. 41. Constantino JN, Gruber CP. Social Responsiveness Scale, 2nd ed. Torrance, CA: Western Psychological Services; 2012. (p. SRS-2).
  42. 42. Constantino JN, Todd RD. Intergenerational transmission of subthreshold autistic traits in the general population. Biol Psychiatry. 2005; 57: 655–660. pmid:15780853
  43. 43. Mazen I, Hiort O, Bassiouny R, El Gammal M. Differential diagnosis of disorders of sex development in Egypt. Horm Res. 2008; 70: 118–123. pmid:18547960
  44. 44. Kaufman AS, Kaufman NL. Kaufman Assessment Battery for Children (K-ABC) administration and scoring manual. Circle Pines, MN: American Guidance Service; 1983.
  45. 45. Kaufman AS, O’Neal MR, Avant AH, Long SW. Introduction to the Kaufman Assessment Battery for Children (K-ABC) for pediatric neuro clinicians. J Child Neurol. 1987; 2: 3–16. pmid:3624826
  46. 46. Wiebe MJ. Test Review: The Kaufman Assessment Battery for Children. J Sch Psychol. 1986; 22: 369–371.
  47. 47. White H. H. W. A heteroskedasticity-consistent covariance matrix estimator and a direct test for heteroskedasticity. Econometrica. 1980; 48: 817–838.
  48. 48. Horita N, Kaneko T. Genetic model selection for a case-control study and a meta-analysis. Meta Gene. 2015; 5: 1–8. pmid:26042205
  49. 49. Bogen JE. The other side of the brain. II. An appositional mind. Bull Los Angeles Neurol Soc. 1969; 34: 135–162. pmid:4897756
  50. 50. Gazzaniga MS. Recent research on hemispheric lateralization of the human brain: Review of the split brain. UCLA Educ. 1975; 17: 9–12.
  51. 51. Nebes RD. Hemispheric specialization in commissurotomized man. Psychol Bull. 1974; 81: 1–14. pmid:4590870
  52. 52. Gordon HW. Music and the right hemisphere. New York: Academic Press; 1983.
  53. 53. Tompkins CA. Rehabilitation for cognitive-communication disorders in right hemisphere brain damage. Arch Phys Med Rehabil. 2012; 93 suppl: S61–S69. pmid:22202193
  54. 54. Ozonoff S, Miller JN. An exploration of right-hemisphere contributions to the pragmatic impairments of autism. Brain Lang. 1996; 52: 411–434. pmid:8653388
  55. 55. Turkstra LS, Clark A, Burgess S, Hengst JA, Wertheimer JC, Paul D. Pragmatic communication abilities in children and adults: implications for rehabilitation professionals. Disabil Rehabil. 2017; 39: 1872–1885. pmid:27540898
  56. 56. Bishop DV. Autism, Asperger’s syndrome and semantic-pragmatic disorder: where are the boundaries? Br J Disord Commun. 1989; 24: 107–121. pmid:2690915
  57. 57. Brook SL, Bowler DM. Autism by another name? Semantic and pragmatic impairments in children. J Autism Dev Disord. 1992; 22: 61–81. pmid:1592765
  58. 58. Shields J. Semantic-pragmatic disorder: a right hemisphere syndrome? Br J Disord Commun. 1991; 26: 383–392. pmid:1814422
  59. 59. Scott-Van Zeeland AA, Abrahams BS, Alvarez-Retuerto AI, Sonnenblick LI, Rudie JD, Ghahremani D, et al. Altered functional connectivity in frontal lobe circuits is associated with variation in the autism risk gene CNTNAP2. Sci Transl Med. 2010; 2: 56ra80. pmid:21048216
  60. 60. Koeda M, Watanabe A, Tsuda K, Matsumoto M, Ikeda Y, Kim W, et al. Interaction effect between handedness and CNTNAP2 polymorphism (rs7794745 genotype) on voice-specific frontotemporal activity in healthy individuals: an fMRI study. Front Behav Neurosci. 2015; 9: 87. pmid:25941478
  61. 61. Wessels JA, Kooloos WM, De Jonge R, De Vries-Bouwstra JK, Allaart CF, Linssen A, et al. Relationship between genetic variants in the adenosine pathway and outcome of methotrexate treatment in patients with recent-onset rheumatoid arthritis. Arthritis Rheum. 2006; 54: 2830–2839. pmid:16947783
  62. 62. Newton-Cheh C, Hirschhorn JN. Genetic association studies of complex traits: design and analysis issues. Mutat Res. 2005; 573: 54–69. pmid:15829237
  63. 63. Kaufman AS, Kaufman NL. Kaufman Assessment Battery for Children Second Edition. Circle Pines, MN: American Guidance Service; 2004.
  64. 64. Kaufman AS, KR W., Kaufman NL. The Kaufman Assessment Battery for Children (K-ABC), In Newmark: Major Psychological Assessment instruments. Boston: Allyn & Bacon; 1985.