Skip to main content
Advertisement
Browse Subject Areas
?

Click through the PLOS taxonomy to find articles in your field.

For more information about PLOS Subject Areas, click here.

  • Loading metrics

Vaccination conspiracy beliefs among social science & humanities and STEM educated people—An analysis of the mediation paths

  • Željko Pavić ,

    Contributed equally to this work with: Željko Pavić, Adrijana Šuljok

    Roles Conceptualization, Data curation, Formal analysis, Funding acquisition, Investigation, Methodology, Project administration, Supervision, Validation, Writing – original draft, Writing – review & editing

    Affiliation Faculty of Humanities and Social Sciences, Josip Juraj Strossmayer University of Osijek, Osijek, Croatia

  • Adrijana Šuljok

    Contributed equally to this work with: Željko Pavić, Adrijana Šuljok

    Roles Conceptualization, Data curation, Formal analysis, Investigation, Methodology, Supervision, Validation, Writing – original draft, Writing – review & editing

    adrijana@idi.hr

    Affiliation Institute for Social Research in Zagreb, Zagreb, Croatia

Abstract

Understanding vaccine hesitancy is becoming increasingly important, especially after the global outbreak of COVID-19. The main goal of this study was to explore the differences in vaccination conspiracy beliefs between people with a university degree coming from different scientific fields—Social Sciences & Humanities (SH) and Science, Technology, Engineering and Mathematics (STEM). The study was conducted on an online convenience sample of respondents with college and university degrees in Croatia (N = 577). The results revealed that respondents educated in SH proved to be more prone to vaccination conspiracy beliefs. The indirect effect through science literacy was confirmed, while this was not the case for the indirect effects through health beliefs (natural immunity beliefs) and trust in the healthcare system. However, all three variables were important direct predictors of vaccination conspiracy beliefs. Female gender and religiosity were positively correlated with vaccination conspiracy beliefs, while age was not a statistically significant predictor. The authors concluded by emphasizing the necessity of the more theoretically elaborated approaches to the study of the educational and other socio-demographic differences in vaccine hesitancy.

Introduction

It has become commonplace to start a research paper that deals with vaccine hesitancy with a paradox that stems from the tension between the well-known and scientifically-proven benefits of vaccination and the rising tide of vaccine hesitancy. The paradox has led to an extensive corpus of research and various models that aimed to explain and systematize the main factors that lie behind vaccine hesitancy. For instance, according to the so-called 3Cs model proposed by the WHO EURO Vaccine Communications Working Group in 2011 and further elaborated by the SAGE Working Group on Vaccine Hesitancy and MacDonald [1], the factors can be divided into three main groups: (a) confidence (trust in vaccine safety and effectiveness, trust in healthcare providers and policy-makers), (b) complacency (health beliefs, perception of vaccine-preventable diseases as risk-free, thus making vaccination unnecessary), and (c) convenience (availability, affordability, accessibility, science and health literacy, etc.). Research confirmed the importance of the elements from the 3Cs model [e.g. 2, 3]. Given the focus of the current study and the multitude of research studies, we will only outline the main findings of selected elements from the 3Cs model pertaining to the institutional trust (confidence), postmodern health beliefs (complacency), and science literacy (convenience—“ability to understand”).

When it comes to the institutional trust, a great number of research studies established a link between trust in social institutions and the healthcare system on one side, and vaccine hesitancy on the other, especially in incident/critical situations. For instance, Lewis and Speers [4] linked the confidence crisis in the MMR vaccine in the UK with the lack of confidence in the executive power at the time, and Mesch and Schwirian [5] found that willingness to vaccinate against H1N1 in the USA during the 2008/2009 pandemic was negatively linked to confidence in the government’s ability to address this health crisis. Orr et al. [6] studying the example of the 2013 Polio outbreak in Israel, found that two of the five most important reasons to resist vaccination were related to trust in the government (general mistrust and the conviction that the government had obtained too many vaccines and wanted to get rid of them by vaccinating children). Similar conclusions about the importance of trust were put forward in other studies [711].

Complacency also contributes to low vaccine acceptance [2]. With regard to the so-called postmodern health beliefs (preference for natural immunity, hesitancy with regard to the use of pharmaceuticals, etc.), Repalust et al. [12] found that the use of complementary and alternative medicine (CAM) was positively associated with vaccine hesitancy in Croatia. Similar correlation was established in the research of Pavić and Milanović [13], where postmodern health beliefs were associated with CAM use and suspicion in conventional medicine in Croatia, whereas other research also confirms the link between such type of health beliefs and vaccine hesitancy [1418]. On the other hand, as shown by Lorini et al. [19], the impact of health literacy, as a specific type of science literacy, on vaccine hesitancy is not clear, given that reliable studies which dealt with the topic were not numerous and that the studies used different measures and brought about mutually opposing results that depend on age, country and vaccine types.

Generally speaking, the relationship between educational level and vaccine hesitancy is non-consistent and thus non-conclusive, even though it seems that most of the research results confirmed negative correlation between educational level and vaccine hesitancy. For example, such results were obtained by Ritvo et al. [20] on a sample of the Canadian general population and Peretti-Wattel et al. [21] in a study conducted in France. On the other hand, Börjesson and Enander [22] determined that in Sweden it was the less educated who were more likely to be vaccinated, while Casiday et al. [23] did not establish the correlation between acceptance of MMR vaccine and education. Numerous other studies can be cited that shows a positive [24, 25] or negative [6, 26, 27] correlation between educational level and pro-vaccination attitudes and behaviors. However, such studies explore educational level in general, probably tacitly assuming that science, health or vaccine literacy/knowledge was the mediating mechanism.

In the current study, we wanted to go beyond just exploring the level of education itself and its correlation to vaccine hesitancy. Bearing in mind the possible epistemic differences between the fields of science, we aimed to examine whether differences in the fields of education (i.e., scientific fields) might influence vaccine hesitancy. In other words, our expectation was that the academic background of individuals educated in social sciences and humanities (hereinafter: SH), that are also known as “soft” and cognitively non-restrictive sciences, differed from the academic background of the individuals educated in natural science, technology, engineering and mathematics (hereinafter: STEM) fields, usually defined as “hard”, cognitively restrictive scientific fields [28]. Consequently, there should be different levels of vaccine hesitancy among individuals with a degree in these fields. Namely, our basic contention was that individuals educated in SH fields might be somewhat more skeptical, that is, more inclined to relativize and point out uncertainties relating to vaccines than individuals educated in STEM fields. Such differences can be the result of the different academic socializations and/or the individual characteristics which are important when choosing a field of study. Additionally, it is our contention that such differences will be mediated by some of the indicators coming from the 3Cs model (trust in the healthcare system, specific health beliefs, and science literacy). The basis for this assumption lies in the concept of socio-cognitive differentiation of scientific fields and their cultures, that is, in the epistemic differences between them, which we will elaborate in the next section of the paper. In addition, up to this moment, there are only few studies which dealt with this research topic, mainly confirming that vaccine hesitancy is higher among SH graduates [2931]. However, the factors behind the established differences are completely unexplored.

Therefore, we proceed with a summary of possible epistemic differences between social sciences and humanities on one side, and science, technology, engineering and mathematics on the other. Then we link these differences to the aforementioned 3Cs factors influencing vaccine hesitancy. In short, we aim to explore selected 3Cs indicators as possible mediating mechanisms arising from the epistemic differences between the scientific fields.

Epistemic differences and vaccine hesitancy

When it comes to fields of science, there have been many disputes in relation to (1) importance and legitimacy of knowledge in different fields, (2) methodological and epistemic standards and their possible unification, and (3) objectivity of the scientific knowledge in general. Some of these differences between the scientific fields can be grounded in a concept called „epistemic cultures”[32] or related concepts that indicate the socio-cognitive differentiation of sciences, that is, scientific fields [33], Becker’s academic tribes and territories [34], Biglan’s distinction of soft and hard scientific fields [28, 35], etc. These concepts, mostly conceived in the 1970s and 1980s and developed later, deal with differences in cognitive “objects”, “styles”, “structures” and “cultures”. If we try to make a rough cross-section of these theoretical approaches, it could be argued that STEM fields are characterized by a more rigid cognitive style, routine research and protocolisation, epistemological realism, an emphasis on the objectivity and superiority of scientific methods and practices, and an understanding of scientific knowledge as a cumulative endeavor. On the other hand, SH fields are characterized by theoretical pluralism, discursiveness, less rigid cognitive style, more open and uncertain findings of research, that is, epistemological relativism and greater criticism of scientific methods and practices.

To illustrate the aforementioned differences, a study conducted on a sample of Croatian scientists in natural and social sciences speaks in support of the differences in the understanding of scientific objectivity between the different fields [36]. In this study it was established that natural scientists were firmly convinced of the objectivity of their disciplines, and that they had high confidence in scientific methods and procedures, wherein they emphasized reproducibility and measurement. In contrast, the author of the study noted that relativism was more common among social scientists, who more often pointed out that subjectivity was somewhat inevitable, showed greater skepticism about the omnipotence of research rules and methods, are more often suspected of the notion that objectivity can be achieved, or even in principle deny the possibility of objectivity [36]. In other words, the author summarized that the inclination to positivism and the idea of the fully objective science can be more often found among researchers coming from natural sciences. On the other hand, different ideas and cultural values were more often present in social sciences and humanities. By definition, these sciences explore the human being and human societies, thus encouraging self-awareness, self-exploration and self-expression that reject the exclusive domination of the medical science (and natural sciences in general) when it comes to delineating what is and what is not a legitimate health issue. On the other hand, STEM educated people are socialized into an epistemic culture that promotes the more objective epistemic standards. For instance, biomedicine-trained individuals embrace the experimental epistemic culture as the „gold standard“, thus often rejecting the social sciences methods, such as surveys and qualitative methods, as insufficiently rigorous [37].

The differences in vaccine hesitancy might be partly explained by a different role that trust and criticism play in the STEM and SH epistemic cultures. Social sciences and humanities in general are characterized by epistemic cultures with high levels of context-dependence and with paying close attention to complexity and uncertainty. In consequence, it can be assumed that the people with an SH degree would be more prone to criticism in relation to scientific objectivity, and therefore less willing to accept a position of a layman who accepts the scientific results without a critical stance. When it comes to the issue of trust in social institutions, as Kuhn notes [38], social sciences find themselves in an ambivalent position as related to the social elites. On the one hand, social sciences share some metaphysical assumptions, such as the assumption of man’s unsocial nature that must be socialized and tamed in order to produce a good society. On the other hand, social sciences reflect critically on current social processes and acknowledge the discrepancy between the social “facts” and the ideal and promised society, wherein particular social institutions and social elites can be heavily criticized for not delivering social services in the expected way.

Research goal and hypotheses

The main goal of the current study was to connect the aforementioned differences between the „hard”and „soft”sciences with vaccine hesitancy. To be more precise, the hitherto research of vaccine hesitancy explored the impact of educational level only in general. However, the above-mentioned considerations lead us to believe that the difference in epistemic cultures might exert a significant impact on vaccine hesitancy, in particular vaccination conspiracy beliefs. Therefore, our goal was to fill in the gaps of the research so far by determining possible differences between science fields/fields of study when it comes to vaccine hesitancy, and to explore proximate cause of such differences. With this in mind, our aim was to explore possible differences in vaccination conspiracy beliefs as a specific type of vaccine hesitancy between university educated people graduating in different science fields. Consequently, our hypotheses were stated as follows:

  1. H1. Respondents who graduated in STEM fields have lower vaccination conspiracy beliefs in comparison to the respondents who graduated in SH fields.
  2. H2. Trust in healthcare system, natural immunity beliefs and science literacy are the mediators of the differences in vaccination conspiracy beliefs described in H1.

Our H2 represents a parallel multiple mediation model which can be visualized by means of the following conceptual diagram (Fig 1). Here we emphasize that in order to preserve visual clarity we omitted control variables (gender, age, and religiosity) from the figure. In other words, it is important to note that the mediation from H2 is estimated while simultaneously controlling the above-mentioned demographic variables. Consequently, the possible mediating influences cannot be attributed to the confounding effects of the demographic variables since they are controlled for in the statistical model.

thumbnail
Fig 1. Conceptual diagram of the parallel mediation tested in the study.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0264722.g001

Methods and instruments

A convenience sample consisted of college and university educated people in Croatia (N = 577), and an online questionnaire was used as the data-collection tool. The participants were recruited with snowball method. The initial sample size was 624, but 47 respondents (7.53%) had missing values on some of the study variables. We conduced binary logistic regression with the missing value as the criterion variable and did not find any significant relationship with other study variables. That is why we proceeded with the complete case analysis, which reduced our sample from 624 to 577 respondents. The data were collected in June 2019 within a pilot study of vaccine hesitancy conducted by the authors. Namely, only the answers of the respondents from the pilot study who graduated from a university were used in the current study. The sample comprised 345 women and 232 men (59.79% and 40.21%, respectively). There were 315 SH and 262 STEM graduates (54.59% and 45.41%).

As for the delineation of SH and STEM fields, the official classification by the Ministry of Science and Education of the Republic of Croatia was used [39].

Informed consent was obtained by means of a written online form. Participants could indicate that they have read the description of the study, were over the age of 18, and that they agreed to participate. Participants did not receive any compensation for completing the survey, Members of the Ethics Committee of the Institute for Social Research Zagreb agreed that the project proposal (Protocol 1112/2017) was in accordance with ethical principles and rules of conduct highlighted in the ISRZ Code of Ethics, as well as the rules of conduct of research in social sciences in general.

As for the measurement scales, vaccination conspiracy beliefs were measured by the scale constructed by Shapiro et al. [40], which comprises seven items. All items were answered on a 5-point scale ranging from 1 = completely disagree to 5 = completely agree. The descriptive statistics for the scale items are presented in Table 1. The results from the items were added in order to get the total summary score (M = 15.11, SD = 8.44; MSH = 16.27; SDSH = 8.83; MSTEM = 13.72; SDSTEM = 7.74; t = 3.70: p = 0.00) to be used in the subsequent analyses with higher values representing higher belief in vaccination conspiracies. Internal consistency of the scale as the measure of reliability (Cronbach’s alpha) was 0.967.

thumbnail
Table 1. Descriptive statistics for the vaccine conspiracy beliefs scale.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0264722.t001

A three-item scale of natural immunity beliefs was taken from the Martin and Petrie [41], Vaccination Attitudes Examination (VAX) scale. Those items were answered on a five-point scale ranging from 1 = completely disagree to 5 = completely agree, as well. These three items were also summed in order to get the total summary score (M = 6.60, SD = 3.63; MSH = 6.92; SDSH = 3.83; MSTEM = 6.23; SDSTEM = 3.34; t = 2.32; p = 0.02) since the internal consistency, as measured by Cronbach’s alpha, was high (0.93). Higher values represent higher belief in natural immunity. The descriptive statistics for the items are shown in Table 2.

thumbnail
Table 2. Descriptive statistics for the natural immunity beliefs scale.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0264722.t002

The trust in healthcare system was measured by using a revised version of the nine-item scale constructed by Shea et al. [42], To be more specific, we left out the item about the equal treatment of patients of all races and ethnicities given that the Croatian society is largely mono-ethnic and mono-racial. All the items were answered on a five-point scale ranging from 1 = completely disagree to 5 = completely agree. Even though Shea et al. [42] confirmed that the scale can be divided into two sub-scales (value congruence and competence), in the current study this scale proved to be one-dimensional with a high level of reliability (Cronbach’s alpha was equal to 0.90). The descriptive statistics for the items is shown in Table 3, whereas the total summary score was also used in subsequent analyses (M = 23.82, SD = 6.37; MSH = 23.28; SDSH = 6.34; MSTEM = 24.46; SDSTEM = 6.35; t = -2.22; p = 0.03) with higher values representing higher trust in healthcare system.

thumbnail
Table 3. Descriptive statistics for the trust in healthcare system scale.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0264722.t003

In the current study the items from the so-called Oxford scale of science literacy [43, 44] were used. The motivation for using a general science literacy scale instead of constructing a specific vaccine knowledge scale lies in our judgement that the specific scale would have to be composed of items that are themselves too „contaminated”with attitudes towards vaccines. For instance, the scale developed and applied by Cvjetkovic et al. [29] contained items about the connection between vaccines and autism and about the dangers of applying multiple vaccines at the same time. These items, although relevant, are strongly intertwined with vaccine attitudes and can be thought of more as a measure of vaccine attitudes than of knowledge. In our view, the Oxford scale measures „non-problematic”items and therefore, in spite of its general nature, is a more valid measure of science knowledge in our case. The Oxford scale arose as a joint effort of several researchers [43, 44] used in the report Science and Engineering Indicators prepared by the U.S. National Science Board. Some of the questions were also used in several Eurobarometer surveys [45, 46]. The scale consisted of 15 questions that probe into knowledge about general science facts, and the respondents have to mark them as „true“, „untrue“, „don’t know”answer. Both incorrect and „don’t know”answers were merged into one category, while correct answers comprised the other category making this variable dichotomous. The frequencies of answers on individual items are given in Table 4. The results were summed in order to obtain the total score (M = 12.30, SD = 2.42; MSH = 11.65; SDSH = 2.60; MSTEM = 13.00; SDSTEM = 2.02; t = -6.82; p = 0.00), with higher values indicating higher science literacy. Internal consistency of the variable (Kuder-Richardson—KR20—coefficient) was reasonably high (0.70).

thumbnail
Table 4. Descriptive statistics for the science literacy scale.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0264722.t004

Finally, sociodemographic variables like age, gender, religiosity were also collected. Age was measured by asking participants’ year of birth and then re-coding it into the age measured in years. Gender was measured as female and male, while religious identification was measured from 1-non religious to 6- very religious. Average age was 39.01 years (min = 20; max = 73; SD = 9.25), while the average result on the 1–6 religiosity scale was 3.03 (SD = 1.61).

All the instruments were translated into Croatian, and then back-translated in order to check the validity of the translation. Appropriate changes were made accordingly.

Results

With the purpose of gaining an initial insight into the bivariate relations between the study variables, in Table 5 we present the intercorrelation matrix. Among other things, it can be observed that the higher level of vaccine conspiracy beliefs can be found among women (r = 0.18; females = 16.33, males = 13.29, Cohen’s d = 0.37), more religious people (r = 0.36), people with higher beliefs in natural immunity (r = 0.81), people with lower trust in healthcare system (r = —0.66), and people with lower scientific literacy (r = —0.48). With respect to the field of study (H1), participants with degrees in social sciences and humanities are more likely to have vaccine conspiracy beliefs (r = 0.18; SH = 16.27, STEM = 13.72, Cohen’s d = 0.31).

thumbnail
Table 5. Intercorrelation matrix (Pearson’s correlation).

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0264722.t005

In order to test total, direct and indirect (mediation) effects of the field of study on vaccine conspiracy beliefs (H2), we used PROCESS, a mediation and moderation SPSS macro written by Hayes [47]. Given that PROCESS is a regression-based model, first we present the regression analysis with conspiracy beliefs as the criterion variable (Table 6).

thumbnail
Table 6. Linear regression analysis with the result on the vaccine conspiracy beliefs scale as the criterion variable.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0264722.t006

In comparison to SH graduates STEM graduates score 0.70 points lower on the vaccine conspiracy beliefs scale. A one-unit increase in the natural immunity beliefs on average raises the results on the vaccine conspiracy belief scale by 1.34 points when all other variables are held constant. A one-unit increase in trust in the healthcare system on average lowers the results on the vaccine conspiracy belief scale by 0.40 points. Finally, a one unit increase in science literacy decreases vaccine conspiracy belief by 0.33 points.

As for the demographic variables (gender, age, and religious identification), we can observe that religious identification is a statistically significant predictor, whereas age and gender are not significant predictors when all other variables are entered into equation. Here we can once more emphasize that there is a bivariate correlation between gender and vaccine conspiracy beliefs, with women scoring 3.04 points higher than men (Cohen’s d = 0.37), but this difference disappears in the multivariate analysis. We can also note that a one-unit increase in religious identification raises the result on the vaccine conspiracy belief scale by 0.50 points when the other variables are held constant.

After the full regression model, we proceeded with the mediation analysis. In Table 7, total, direct and indirect effects of field of study are presented, with age, gender and religiosity entered as covariates. The covariates were used in order to check for the possible spurious effects of our predictors. As suggested by Hayes [47], bootstrapping was used as a method of obtaining confidence intervals of the estimates of the indirect effects and the pairwise differences between indirect effects.

thumbnail
Table 7. Vaccine conspiracy beliefs—Mediation analysis.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0264722.t007

We can observe that the total effect coefficient amounts to—2.18, meaning that STEM graduates score 2.18 point lower on the vaccine conspiracy beliefs scale when compared to SH graduates, and when both direct and indirect effects are accounted for. The coefficient for total effect can be decomposed into the direct effect (- 0.70) and the indirect (mediation) effect (-1.45), with the latter including indirect effects of natural immunity beliefs (- 0.71), trust in healthcare system (- 0.37) and science literacy (- 0.39). The direct effect is not statistically significant, while only the indirect effect of scientific literacy proved to be significant by means of the bootstrapping analysis. In the remaining two cases, the bootstrapping analysis of indirect (mediating) effects revealed that zero effect is included into the confidence intervals. All bootstrapping confidence intervals of the pairwise differences between mediation effects also contained zero difference, thus not being able to confirm the existence of the pairwise differences between the indirect effects.

Discussion

In pursuance of the research goal, in this paper we proposed two hypotheses. The first one (H1) was related to the differences in vaccination conspiracy beliefs between SH and STEM fields, and the second one (H2) to the possible mediating mechanism of such differences.

To summarize the findings pertaining to H1, even though both groups mostly largely reject vaccination conspiracy beliefs, our findings confirm that the people with an SH degree are more prone to vaccination conspiracy beliefs than the people with a STEM degree, thus confirming H1. The bivariate difference equals 2.55 points, it falls to 0.70 when demographic variables (gender, age, and religiosity) are accounted for, but it also rises to 2.18 when both direct and indirect effects are included. Bearing in mind the lack of similar studies, we cannot directly compare our results with other research. We can confirm that our results are in concordance with the results obtained by Cvjetkovic et al. [29], who determined that the law students of the Belgrade University (Serbia) had substantially more negative vaccine attitudes than the engineering, and especially medical students, even while controlling for vaccine knowledge. However, since Cvjetkovic et al. did not conduct a full mediation analysis, their results cannot be directly compared to our results with regard to the indirect effects. Likewise, a study conducted by Šálek et al. [31] on a sample of medical and teacher education students in the Czech Republic found differences in the positive vaccine perception rate between those two groups. Vaccine acceptance rate was lower among teacher education students (72%) than among medical students (92%). Also teacher education students more often reported negative experiences with vaccination and were more often alternative medicine followers.

On the other hand, the mediation analysis conducted within our study confirmed the mediation effect of scientific literacy, while the mediation effect of natural immunity beliefs and the trust in healthcare system were not reliably confirmed, thus only partially confirming our second hypothesis (H2). Notwithstanding that fact, we emphasize that all mediation factors are important direct predictors of vaccine conspiracy beliefs. The data employed in the study just did not confirm the mediation function between the field of study and vaccination conspiracy beliefs in case of the two mediators. Another word of caution is also warranted here. As mentioned before, a parallel multiple mediation model with control variables was tested in our study. This means that mediation paths were tested with simultaneously controlling the effects of gender, religiosity and age. In additional analyses we confirmed that all mediation paths were statistically significant when gender was not employed as a control variable, while in that case indirect effects of natural immunity beliefs, trust in the healthcare system and science literacy amount to—0.87,—0.43 and—0.42, respectively. Consequently, the mediation paths can be explained by the disproportionate presence of women in STEM fields and the correlations between gender on one side, and vaccine conspiracy beliefs and the mediating factors on the other side, as visible from the intercorrelation matrix. In other words, all three mediators are indeed significant, but in cases of two of them (trust in healthcare system and natural immunity beliefs) this is the case only because women on average happen to have lower trust and higher beliefs in natural immunity, as well as higher vaccination conspiracy beliefs, while at the same time being disproportionally more present in social sciences and humanities.

The direct impact of the natural immunity preference on vaccine conspiracy beliefs is consistent with the results of the study of vaccine hesitancy in Croatia [12], which showed that the use of complementary and alternative medicine (CAM) was positively connected to vaccine hesitancy and vaccine refusal, as well as with a research conducted on a sample of Croatian population [13] that showed a negative connection between „postmodern”views on health (natural immunity included) and trust in conventional medicine as opposed to the trust in CAM. Positive attitudes towards CAM and its use partially follow from the general question of the legitimacy of the medical science authority. Namely, as Sointu [48] summarized, CAM users defy the biomedical definition of health by arguing for holistic health that empowers them by providing a sense of agency, control and meaning. Therefore, CAM produces a new selfhood and subjectivity that is directed towards exploring the inner-self. However, in the current study we could not reliably confirm that such differences represent a mediating factor of the established connection between the field of study and vaccination conspiracy beliefs. Namely, although the mediation effect was present in the hypothesized direction, its statistical significance could not be demonstrated by the bootstrapping analysis. Nevertheless, given the theoretical plausibility of such differences in epistemic culture and the fact that the bootstrapping interval included zero only by a small margin, we think that new studies which will employ random sampling are warranted.

Similarly, we also could not reliably confirm the mediation effect of the trust in the healthcare system with our study data. The possible explanation might be that the aforementioned criticism penetrated all fields of study and that it does not explain the differences in vaccine hesitancy. Perhaps we should not overstate the differences/gap between natural sciences and social sciences/humanities, since objectivity is nevertheless an important scientific ideal in (all) sciences [49]. Especially in the social sciences, methods and precision of natural sciences continue to be an inspiration to be emulated for some of the scientists from this field, but who are still aware of a distinctiveness of the social sciences. Conversely, in their qualitative study of science and non-science (i.e. humanities) tertiary educated people, Quinn et al. [50] established that even people who accepted the scientific method of evidence-based medicine tended to exhibit various „habits of mind“, such as the mistrust of authority, open-mindedness, skepticism, and rationality/belief systems that led them to engage in the acceptance and use of CAM. Thus, as Quin et al. concluded, scientific literacy can go hand in hand with belief systems that are prone to potential distrust towards science and scientists. In other words, a high level of science knowledge is not always translated into support for science [51], that is, individuals can use knowledge in different ways, according to their pre-existing interests and motivations [52], or educational epistemic culture. Additionally, it should be emphasized that within the scientific fields there are considerable differences among the various disciplines, and that there are various paradigms and methodological approaches within the same discipline. However, despite the fact that our mediation analysis did not confirm the impact of this factor, we think that the idea should be tested in future studies conducted on different samples, given that the bootstrapping interval from our study in this case as well comprised zero only by a small margin.

We posited that the aforementioned epistemic differences and the obvious assumption that people with a degree in SH should be less familiar with the knowledge coming from natural sciences, biomedical sciences included, could also explain the differences in vaccine hesitancy. In their meta-analysis of the connection between science literacy and science attitudes, after controlling for several potential confounding variables, Allum et al. [53] found a „small but consistent”correlation that gave some support to the deficit model of the relationship between science knowledge and attitudes. A study in Croatia on a representative sample also indicated a significant but weak relationship between the level of scientific literacy and (positive) attitudes toward science [54]. This is consistent with the results of the current study, in that science literacy is a statistically significant predictor of vaccine conspiracy beliefs, as well as the mediator of the SH-STEM differences in vaccine hesitancy. In other words, the results of the current study confirm the above mentioned “small, but consistent” relationship, given a partial mediation of the differences in the vaccine conspiracy beliefs between the STEM and SH fields through science literacy.

Aside from the discussion pertaining to H1 and H2, we shall also present a short discussion of the results with regard to other sociodemographic variables. We should, however, bear in mind that our study was conducted on a sample of higher-educated individuals, thus not being fully comparable to the results obtained in studies on general population research samples.

To start with, we have not confirmed the relationship between age and vaccine conspiracy beliefs. However, this relationship is highly varied and far from being conclusive. For instance, our results differ from the results obtained by Repalust et al. [12] since in their study the younger age groups were less vaccine acceptant in comparison to the older age groups. However, other research can be cited that determined a positive [55], negative [56], or non-existent [57] relation between age and various measures of vaccine hesitancy. These differences might be the result of age differences in relation to education, health beliefs, trust in social institutions etc. However, to our best knowledge, age differences in vaccination attitudes so far have not been comprehensively theoretically explained.

With regard to the established positive connection between religiosity and vaccine conspiracy beliefs, the results of the current study are mainly consistent with the results obtained by other studies [12]. Possible reasons for the relationship between religiosity and negative vaccine attitudes might include a latent conflict between science and religiosity within the Croatian citizens’ value system or, more specifically, the framing of biomedical questions as religious issues, dominated by the idea that medical science should not meddle too much into „God’s work“. More specifically, religious people might object to the fact that human and animal tissues, such as cell lines from aborted fetuses and hydrolyzed gelatin obtained from animals, are being used in the production of some vaccines [58]. As for the Catholic Church, being the dominant religious community in the Croatian society, according to the document drafted in 2005 by the Pontifical Academy for Life, the use of vaccines with the aborted fetuses’ tissue constitutes „at least a mediate remote passive material cooperation to the abortion“. Catholics are thus obliged to push for morally acceptable alternatives in the vaccine production, even though the use of vaccines should be tolerated in order to save children’s lives. Such a situation presents Catholic parents with a „moral coercion”that must be resolved as soon as possible [59].

When it comes to gender, our results are in line with the majority of studies indicating that vaccine hesitancy is more likely to be found among women [17, 60], even though inverse findings can also be found [61]. In addition, the results of our study suggest an interesting finding that the gender composition of scientific fields can be a part of the explanation of their epistemic differences and its impact on vaccine hesitancy. These results are in line with preponderance of women in the CAM, both as users and practitioners [62], as well as with the positive relationship between CAM beliefs and practices and vaccine hesitancy [63].

Conclusion and limitations

The results of our preliminary study suggest that future research on vaccine hesitancy should differentiate not only between different education levels, but between education/science fields as well. Namely, the people with a degree in SH proved to be more prone to vaccination conspiracy beliefs, whereas this difference is probably mediated at least by the differences in science literacy. The reasons could probably be found in the different academic socialization and the background characteristics which are important when selecting a field of study. Future studies should also further explore the possible mediators of this difference and establish some more in-depth theoretical explanations for their link with epistemic differences between scientific fields. In other words, when it comes to education and other sociodemographic factors as well, more comprehensive research is warranted that will go beyond simple descriptions of sociodemographic differences in vaccine hesitancy, in order to draw more complete conclusions, as well as possible recommendations in terms of science/health education.

Even though we did not confirm that beliefs in natural immunity and trust in the healthcare system are mediating paths of the vaccination conspiracy beliefs differences between SH and STEM, these constructs proved to be strong predictors of vaccination conspiracy beliefs. In the context of the COVID-19 pandemics this finding suggests that vaccine hesitancy cannot be contested only by providing factual, scientific information on COVID-19 vaccines. Successful COVID-19 crisis management needs to include a trust-building communication, and to address lay beliefs about vaccine and health in general.

In our view, the main weakness of the current study represents the convenience sample employed, as well as the data-collection method (online survey) employed in the study. It can be reasonably assumed that some amount of self-selection among potential study participants might be present in the study. Therefore, we cannot exclude the possibility that some sort of bias is shared between the study variables that might influence their relationship. It might also be assumed that the variance of the measurements was somewhat restricted, that is, the less (or more) vaccine hesitant people might have been more inclined to participate in the survey which might have influenced our results and conclusions. As for the measurements employed in the study, more specific measures of vaccine knowledge might bring different results than general measures of science knowledge such as the Oxford scale used in the current study. In addition, given that our study was cross-sectional and the possible omitted variables bias might have existed, causal mechanisms cannot be fully supported by the data from our study.

References

  1. 1. MacDonald NE, Eskola J, Liang X, Chaudhuri M, Dube E, Gellin B, et al. Vaccine hesitancy: Definition, scope and determinants. Vaccine 2015. 33(34): 4161–4164. pmid:25896383
  2. 2. Quinn S, Jamison A, Musa D, Hilyard K, Freimuth V. Exploring the Continuum of Vaccine Hesitancy Between African American and White Adults: Results of a Qualitative Study. PLoS Curr. 2016 Dec 29;8:ecurrents.outbreaks.3e4a5ea39d8620494e2a2c874a3c4201. pmid:28239512.
  3. 3. Schmid P, Rauber D, Betsch C, Lidolt G, Denker ML. Barriers of Influenza Vaccination Intention and Behavior—A Systematic Review of Influenza Vaccine Hesitancy, 2005–2016. PLoS One. 2017 Jan 26;12(1):e0170550. pmid:28125629.
  4. 4. Lewis J, Speers T. Misleading media reporting? The MMR story. Nat Rev Immunol 2003. 3(11): 913–918. pmid:14608376
  5. 5. Mesch GS, Schwirian KP. Confidence in government and vaccination willingness in the USA. Health Promot Int 2015. 30(2): 213–221. pmid:25369794
  6. 6. Orr D, Baram-Tsabari A, Landsman K. Social media as a platform for health-related public debates and discussions: The Polio vaccine on Facebook. Isr J Health Policy Res 2016. 5:34. pmid:27843544
  7. 7. Brownlie J, Howson A. ‘Leaps of Faith’ and MMR: An Empirical Study of Trust. Sociology. 2005;39(2):221–239.
  8. 8. Craciun C, Baban A. "Who will take the blame?": understanding the reasons why Romanian mothers decline HPV vaccination for their daughters. Vaccine. 2012 Nov 6;30(48):6789–93. Epub 2012 Sep 24. pmid:23017603.
  9. 9. Hobson-West P. ’Trusting blindly can be the biggest risk of all’: organised resistance to childhood vaccination in the UK. Sociol Health Illn. 2007 Mar;29(2):198–215. pmid:17381813.
  10. 10. Hornsey MJ, Lobera J, Díaz-Catalán C. Vaccine hesitancy is strongly associated with distrust of conventional medicine, and only weakly associated with trust in alternative medicine. Soc Sci Med. 2020 Jun;255:113019. Epub 2020 May 5. pmid:32408085.
  11. 11. Jamison AM, Quinn SC, Freimuth VS. "You don’t trust a government vaccine": Narratives of institutional trust and influenza vaccination among African American and white adults. Soc Sci Med. 2019 Jan;221:87–94. Epub 2018 Dec 12. pmid:30576982.
  12. 12. Repalust A, Šević S, Rihtar S, Štulhofer A. Childhood vaccine refusal and hesitancy intentions in Croatia: insights from a population-based study. Psychol Heal Med 2017. 22(9):1045–1055. pmid:27899030
  13. 13. Pavić Ž, Milanović G. Komplementarna i alternativna medicina u Hrvatskoj: testiranje triju hipoteza (Complementary and alternative medicine in Croatia: A test of three hypotheses). Soc Ekol 2014. 23(2): 95–119.
  14. 14. Attwell K, Ward PR, Meyer SB, Rokkas PJ, Leask J. "Do-it-yourself": Vaccine rejection and complementary and alternative medicine (CAM). Soc Sci Med. 2018 Jan;196:106–114. Epub 2017 Nov 16. pmid:29175699.
  15. 15. Frawley JE, McIntyre E, Wardle J, Jackson D. Is there an association between the use of complementary medicine and vaccine uptake: results of a pilot study. BMC Res Notes. 2018 Apr 2;11(1):217. pmid:29609640.
  16. 16. Hadjipanayis A, van Esso D, del Torso S, Dornbusch HJ, Michailidou K, Minicuci N, et al. Vaccine confidence among parents: Large scale study in eighteen European countries. Vaccine 2020. 38(6): 1505–1512. pmid:31848051
  17. 17. Peretti-Watel P, Larson HJ, Ward JK, Schulz WS, Verger P. Vaccine hesitancy: Clarifying a theoretical framework for an ambiguous notion. PLoS Curr 2015. pmid:25789201
  18. 18. Soveri A, Karlsson LC, Antfolk J, Lindfelt M, Lewandowsky S. Unwillingness to engage in behaviors that protect against COVID-19: the role of conspiracy beliefs, trust, and endorsement of complementary and alternative medicine. BMC Public Health. 2021 Apr 8;21(1):684. pmid:33832446.
  19. 19. Lorini C, Santomauro F, Grazzini M, Mantwill S, Vettori V, Lastrucci V, et al. Health literacy in Italy: A cross-sectional study protocol to assess the health literacy level in a population-based sample, and to validate health literacy measures in the Italian language. BMJ Open 2017. pmid:29138204
  20. 20. Ritvo P, Irvine J, Klar N, Wilson K, Brown L, Bremner KE, et al. A Canadian national survey of attitudes and knowledge regarding preventive vaccines. J Immune Based Ther Vaccines 2003. 1(1):3 pmid:14613575
  21. 21. Peretti-Watel P, Verger P, Raude J, Constant A, Gautier A, Jestin C, et al. Dramatic change in public attitudes towards vaccination during the 2009 influenza A(H1N1) pandemic in France. Eurosurveillance 2013. pmid:24176658
  22. 22. Börjesson M, Enander A. Perceptions and sociodemographic factors influencing vaccination uptake and precautionary behaviours in response to the A/H1N1 influenza in Sweden. Scand J Public Health 2014. 42(2): 215–222. pmid:24259541
  23. 23. Casiday R, Cresswell T, Wilson D, Panter-Brick C. A survey of UK parental attitudes to the MMR vaccine and trust in medical authority. Vaccine 2006. 24(2): 177–184. pmid:16157422
  24. 24. Mora T, Trapero-Bertran M. The influence of education on the access to childhood immunization: The case of Spain. BMC Public Health 2018. pmid:30021538
  25. 25. Shui IM, Weintraub ES, Gust DA. Parents Concerned About Vaccine Safety. Differences in Race/Ethnicity and Attitudes. Am J Prev Med 2006. 31(3):244–51. pmid:16905036
  26. 26. Steens A, Stefanoff P, Daae A, Vestrheim DF, Riise Bergsaker MA. High overall confidence in childhood vaccination in Norway, slightly lower among the unemployed and those with a lower level of education. Vaccine 2020. 38(29): 4536–4541. pmid:32448621
  27. 27. Yang Y, Delamater PL, Leslie TF, Mello MM. Sociodemographic predictors of vaccination exemptions on the basis of personal belief in California. Am J Public Health 2016. 106(1): 172–177. pmid:26562114
  28. 28. Biglan A. The characteristics of subject matter in different academic areas. J Appl Psychol 1973. 57(3), 195–203.
  29. 29. Cvjetkovic SJ, Jeremic VL, Tiosavljevic DV. Knowledge and attitudes toward vaccination: A survey of Serbian students. J Infect Public Health 2017. 10(5): 649–656. pmid:28669785
  30. 30. Sallam M, Dababseh D, Yaseen A, Al-Haidar A, Ababneh NA, Bakri FG, et al. Conspiracy Beliefs Are Associated with Lower Knowledge and Higher Anxiety Levels Regarding COVID-19 among Students at the University of Jordan. Int J Environ Res Public Health. 2020 Jul 8;17(14):4915. pmid:32650409.
  31. 31. Šálek J, Čelko AM, Dáňová J. Vaccination perception and attitude among undergraduate medical and teacher education students at Charles university, Prague, Czech republic. Vaccines 2020. 8(1):136. pmid:32204313
  32. 32. Knorr Cetina K. Epistemic cultures. How the sciences make knowledge. Cambridge MA: Harvard University Press 1999.
  33. 33. Bourdieu P. The specificity of the scientific field and the social conditions of the progress of reason. Soc Sci Inf 1975. 14 (6):19–47.
  34. 34. Becker T. Academic Tribes and Territories: Intellectual Enquiry and the Culture of the Disciplines. Buckingham: Open University Press 1989.
  35. 35. Biglan A. Relationships between subject matter characteristics and the structure and output of university departments. J Appl Psychol 1973. 57(3): 204–213.
  36. 36. Prpić K. Scientists’ concepts of scientific objectivity. In Prpić, Katarina (ed). Beyond the Myths about the Natural and Social Sciences: A Sociological View 2009. Zagreb: Institut za društvena istraživanja u Zagrebu, 275–321.
  37. 37. Albert M, Laberge S, Hodges BD, Regehr G, Lingard L. Biomedical scientists’ perception of the social sciences in health research. Soc Sci Med 2008. 66(12): 2520–2531. pmid:18336978
  38. 38. Kuhn M. How the Social Sciences Think about World’s Social. Outline of a Critique. Stuttgart: Ibidem Press. 2016.
  39. 39. Pravilnik o znanstvenim i umjetničkim područjima, poljima i granama. https://narodne-novine.nn.hr/clanci/sluzbeni/2009_09_118_2929.html
  40. 40. Shapiro GK, Holding A, Perez S, Amsel R, Rosberger Z. Validation of the vaccine conspiracy beliefs scale. Papillomavirus Res 2016. 2: 167–172. pmid:29074176
  41. 41. Martin LR, Petrie KJ. Understanding the Dimensions of Anti-Vaccination Attitudes: the Vaccination Attitudes Examination (VAX) Scale. Ann Behav Med 2017. pmid:28255934
  42. 42. Shea JA, Micco E, Dean LT, McMurphy S, Schwartz JS, Armstrong K. Development of a revised health care system distrust scale. J Gen Intern Med 2008. 23(6):727–32. pmid:18369678
  43. 43. Durant JR, Evans GA, Thomas GP. The public understanding of science. Nature 1989. 340(6228), 11–14. pmid:2739718
  44. 44. Miller JD. The measurement of civic scientific literacy. Public Underst Sci 1998. 7(1): 203–223.
  45. 45. Special Eurobarometer 224: Europeans, Science and Techonology, survey and report, [Internet]. European Commission, Directorate-General for Communication; 2005 [cited 2022 Jan 24]. http://ec.europa.eu/public_opinion/archives/ebs/ebs_224_en.pdf
  46. 46. Special Eurobarometer 516: European citizens’ knowledge and attitudes towards science and technology [Internet]. European Commission, Directorate-General for Communication; 2021 [cited 2022 Jan 24]. http://data.europa.eu/88u/dataset/S2237_95_2_516_ENG
  47. 47. Hayes A. Introduction to mediation, moderation, and conditional process analysis (Methodology in the Social Sciences) (The Guilford Press, New York, NY). 2013. 978-1-60918-230-4.
  48. 48. Sointu E. Theorizing Complementary and Alternative Medicines. Wellbeing, Self, Gender, Class. Houndmills: Palgrave Macmillan 2012.
  49. 49. Fuchs S. Sociological Theory of Objectivity. Science Studies 1997. 11 (1):4–26.
  50. 50. Quinn F, Taylor N, Coll RK, McClune W. Investigating Views of Science and Humanities: Tertiary Educated Adults on Complementary and Alternative Medicines. Int J Sci Soc 2016. 8(3): 31–50.
  51. 51. Roberts MR, Reid G, Schroeder M, Norris SP. Causal or spurious? The relationship of knowledge and attitudes to trust in science and technologyausal or spurious? The relationship of knowledge and attitudes to trust in science and technology. Public Underst Sci 2013. 22(5): 624–641. pmid:23833175
  52. 52. Allum N, Sibley E, Sturgis P, Stoneman P. Religious beliefs, knowledge about science and attitudes towards medical genetics. Public Underst Sci 2014. 23(7): 833–849. pmid:23838683
  53. 53. Allum N, Sturgis P, Tabourazi D, Brunton-Smith I. Science knowledge and attitudes across cultures: A meta-analysis. Public Underst Sci 2008. 17(1): 35–54.
  54. 54. Šuljok A. Znanstvena pismenost i stavovi prema znanosti u Hrvatskoj. Soc i Pr, 2020 58 (1): 85–111.
  55. 55. Krishnamoorthy Y, Kannusamy S, Sarveswaran G, Majella M, Sarkar S, Narayanan V. Factors related to vaccine hesitancy during the implementation of Measles-Rubella campaign 2017 in rural Puducherry-A mixed-method study. J Fam Med Prim Care 2019. 8(12): 3962–3970. pmid:31879644
  56. 56. Pew Research Center. Public and Scientists’ Views on Science and Society 2015. https://www.pewresearch.org/internet/wp-content/uploads/sites/9/2015/01/PI_ScienceandSociety_Report_012915.pdf
  57. 57. My C, Danchin M, Willaby HW, Pemberton S, Leask J. Parental attitudes, beliefs, behaviours and concerns towards childhood vaccinations in Australia: A national online survey. Aust Fam Physician 2017. 46(3): 145–151. pmid:28260278
  58. 58. Wombwell E, Fangman MT, Yoder AK, Spero DL. Religious Barriers to Measles Vaccination. J Community Health 2015. 40(3): 597–604. pmid:25315714
  59. 59. Pontifical Academy for Life. Moral reflections on vaccines prepared from cells derived from aborted human fetuses 2015. http://www.catholic.org/featured/headline.php?ID=2410, accessed on the 5th of January 2018.
  60. 60. Ren J, Wagner AL, Zheng A, Sun X, Boulton ML, Huang Z, et al. The demographics of vaccine hesitancy in Shanghai, China. PLoS One 2018. pmid:30543712
  61. 61. Luyten J, Bruyneel L, van Hoek AJ. Assessing vaccine hesitancy in the UK population using a generalized vaccine hesitancy survey instrument. Vaccine 2019. 37(18): 2494–2501. pmid:30940484
  62. 62. Cant S, Watts P. Complementary and alternative medicine: Gender and marginality. Palgrave Handb. Gend. Heal. Second Ed., 2012.
  63. 63. Bleser WK, Elewonibi BR, Miranda PY, BeLue R. Complementary and alternative medicine and influenza vaccine uptake in US children. Pediatrics 2016. pmid:27940756