Skip to main content
Advertisement
Browse Subject Areas
?

Click through the PLOS taxonomy to find articles in your field.

For more information about PLOS Subject Areas, click here.

  • Loading metrics

Individual differences in self-reported lie detection abilities

Abstract

Previous literature on lie detection abilities bears an interesting paradox. On the group level, people detect others’ lies at guessing level. However, when asked to evaluate their own abilities, people report being able to detect lies (i.e., self-reported lie detection). Understanding this paradox is important because decisions which rely on credibility assessment and deception detection can have serious implications (e.g., trust in others, legal issues). In two online studies, we tested whether individual differences account for variance in self-reported lie detection abilities. We assessed personality traits (Big-Six personality traits, Dark Triad), empathy, emotional intelligence, cultural values, trust level, social desirability, and belief in one’s own lie detection abilities. In both studies, mean self-reported lie detection abilities were above chance level. Then, lower out-group trust and higher social desirability levels predicted higher self-reported lie detection abilities. These results suggest that social trust and norms shape our beliefs about our own lie detection abilities.

Introduction

Evolutionary approaches highlight that living in social groups is key to being successful in our environments [1]. Living in groups comes not only with advantages though. Firstly, we must assume that other group members are trustworthy and bare no threat, so that we can engage in effective communication. Without trust, we would constantly experience problematic relationships within and between social groups. Interpersonal trust ensures individuals’ well-being, the achievement of their own aims, and the longevity of social units such as families, communities, and institutions [24]. Yet, being excessively trustworthy also bears risks, because others might take advantage for their own benefits [4].

In an ideal world, one would always know who to trust and who not to trust, who and what to believe and not to believe. This ideal world, however, does not exist, because deception occurs daily [5, 6], on average, at least once or twice a day [79]. When people lie, they make completely or partially untrue statements with the intent to deceive, and about 80% of the times, people succeed with lies remaining undetected [7]. Yet, not everybody lies to the same extent with more recent studies showing that a few prolific liars are telling the majority of lies [6, 10, 11]. Then, there is not one type of lies. There are many: lies can be about feelings, opinions, achievements, failures, to name a few examples. We may lie because honesty would hamper social or material goals [7], for financial or emotional gains, to avoid embarrassment, or to protect the feelings of others [7, 1214].

At this point, we wish to highlight a striking paradox between actual (objective) and self-reported (subjective) lie detection abilities. Experimental studies showed that objectively, people perform around chance level when trying to spot somebody else lying [15, 16]. Subjectively, however, people report being able to detect lies successfully [13, 17, 18]. This paradox might originate from the so-called truth-default state, meaning, one assumes that others are honest. Indeed, most everyday situations inspire trust, or at least, do not merit questioning. Such a truth-default state is desirable because trust facilitates cooperation and effective communication [19, 20]. Clare and Levine [19] argued that individuals would only deviate from this truth-default state subsequent to a trigger events, such as suspicious indicators or motives, dishonest behavior, information from third parties, or inconsistencies in statements or known facts.

Overall, we can assume that lying is omnipresent, but that most lies remain undetected, because people see few reasons to distrust each other. Consequently, individuals encounter few opportunities to study and learn about others’ successful lies. Thus, we face an imbalance between i) the number of undetected lies by oneself, ii) the number of undetected lies by others, and iii) the number of a few occasions when lies were detected. This imbalance may leave individuals with an inflated estimation of their own lie detection abilities, once a lie had been detected [17]. Moreover, this inflated estimation might be further supported by people’s tendency to think positively about themselves: being easily deceived would not match this positive self-view [21].

When trying to understand this inflated estimation, we noted a surprising void of published studies reporting on reasons or correlates of self-reported lie detection abilities [2225]. The few available studies focused on the Dark Triad personality traits–Machiavellianism, narcissism, and psychopathy,–because these traits share malevolent behavioral tendencies such as self-promotion, manipulation (including lying), and lack of empathy [2125]. Wissing and Reinhard [22] showed that higher psychopathy and higher overall scores on Dark Triad personality were associated with higher self-reported confidence in lie detection abilities. Then, Zvi and Elaad [23] showed that higher levels of narcissism were associated with higher production of lies, which in turn was associated with higher self-reported lie detection abilities in comparison to others [23]. The recent study by Elaad [25] supported the association between higher narcissism and higher self-reported lie detection abilities in comparison to others. Interestingly, self-reported confidence in one’s own lie detection abilities was unrelated to individuals’ actual lie detection abilities [26]. Also, variance in Dark Triad traits did not explain variance in actual lie detection abilities either [26].

Results on these few studies were not conclusive. Thus, we designed a new study on individual differences on self-reported lie detection abilities. In addition to the Dark Triad personality traits, we included further measures that had been studied in the context of lie detection abilities and lie production before, though again showing inconclusive results. To this end, we selected a series of different measures, namely gender, social cognition measures, cultural differences, general personality traits, and social desirability.

For gender, studies showed no differences between men and women in both lie detection and lie production [2729]. For social cognition, we considered empathy and emotional intelligence. For instance, Duran et al. [30] found that women with lower as compared to higher levels of empathy performed better at lie detection. Former studies would have predicted the opposite, though [31, 32]. Others argued that higher levels of empathy should link to impaired lie detection performances [33, 34]. In respect to emotional intelligence, higher trait levels might facilitate lie detection [35, 36]. Actual studies, however, showed that higher emotional intelligence, particularly emotionality, yielded impaired lie detection abilities [37].

For cultural differences, the need for lie detection might differ as a function of where you live on the globe, because levels of trust, deception, and corruption vary cross-culturally [4, 9, 38]. For instance, in case of lower national trust levels, people might be more suspicious towards others, having a lower truth-default state [20]. Maybe, they perform above chance in lie detection, or they just think that they do, once the odd lie had been detected. Moreover, individuals from collectivistic societies (prioritizing harmony among group members) seem more inclined to lie for other people’s benefits than those from individualistic societies [39].

To account for individual differences, we performed two consecutive online studies to investigate self-reported lie detection abilities. In Study 1, participants completed self-report measures on the Dark Triad [40], empathy [41], cultural values [42], and national trust levels [43]. Participants also indicated whether they were able to detect lies (yes/no), and at which level of accuracy (0–25%, 25–50%, 50–75%, 75–100%). Around 80% of the sample answered yes, with little variance in the accuracy level ratings. Thus, we made several changes to Study 2. We again asked participants to complete some of the same questionnaires (empathy, cultural values, national trust levels), but exchanged the 12-item Dirty Dozen [40] with the psychometrically superior Dark Triad personality traits questionnaire [44]. Additionally, we assessed participants’ general personality traits such as Agreeableness, Openness to Experience, Extraversion, Conscientiousness, Honesty and Resiliency [45], due to formerly described relationships with Dark Triad traits [46, 47]. We also assessed emotional intelligence [48] and socially desirable responding [49]. Crucially, participants again completed the yes/no question regarding their self-reported lie detection abilities. This time they rated their estimated accuracy on a scale from 0 (unable to detect lies) to 100 (perfectly able to detect lies). Moreover, to account for previous studies on the subject (see also [18, 2325, 50, 51], we also asked participants to judge their lie detection abilities in comparison to others on a scale ranging from 0 (much worse than others) to 100 (much better than others).

Across these two studies, we investigated whether we might observe enhanced self-reported lie detection abilities as a function of i) Dark Triad personality scores (i.e., narcissism, psychopathy, and Machiavellianism), ii) lower empathy scores, iii) lower emotional intelligence scores, and iv) cultural values. Regarding cultural values, we expected to find differences between Hofstede’s cultural values (i.e., collectivism) and self-reported lie detection abilities. Finally, we expected self-reported lie detection abilities to be positively associated with social desirability, in line with people’s tendency to think and show themselves in a positive way [21].

Study 1

Method

Participants.

We recruited 764 participants (105 males). After excluding incomplete data and selecting participants between 18 and 31 years old (i.e., the majority), our final sample consisted in 487 participants (99 males) with a mean age of 21.50 years (SDage = 6.57 years; range = 18–31 years). Of these, 418 (95 males) were undergraduate students at the University of Lausanne, Switzerland, who received course credit for their participation. The remaining 69 participants (4 males) were recruited at the University of Franche-Comté, France. All participants were native French speakers. These studies were conducted in accordance with the principles expressed in the Declaration of Helsinki and received approval from the Research Ethics Commission of the Institute of Psychology, University of Lausanne (C_SSP_052021_00001).

We had used the statistical power analysis tool G*Power [52] to estimate the minimum sample size of 473 for a two-tailed binary logistic regression with a small effect size (odds ratio of 1.68) (see[53]), the H0 probability for Y = 1 of .45, α of 0.05, power (1−β) of 0.80.

Material

Self-report questionnaires.

All our questionnaires demonstrated good or acceptable internal consistency (Cronbach’s alpha values between .510 and .817) (see Table 1).

Dark Triad Dirty Dozen inventory [54] (French-Canadian version from Savard et al.

[40]). This short 12-item scale assesses psychopathy, narcissism and Machiavellianism. Participants indicated their agreement with each statement on a 5-point Likert scale (see Table 1). Narcissism items assess the search for admiration and attention, the importance of status and expectations of special favours from others (e.g., “I tend to want others to admire me”). Machiavellianism items assess the use of deception, manipulation, flattery, and exploitation of others to achieve own goals (e.g., “I tend to manipulate others to get my way”). Psychopathy items assess the lack of remorse, insensitivity, being cynical, and being unconcerned by the morality of own actions (e.g., “I tend to lack remorse”). We averaged the scores of the four items of each subscale to obtain the total score on each subscale. Higher scores indicate higher expressions of narcissistic, Machiavellian, or psychopathic traits.

Individual Cultural Values Scale [55] (French version from Zheng [42]).

This 26-item scale is based on Hofstede’s cultural dimensions model [56, 57] assessing the five cultural dimensions of i) power distance, ii) uncertainty avoidance, iii) collectivism vs individualism iv) masculinity, and v) long-term vs. short-term orientation (see Table 1 for details on the scale and sub-scales). Power distance refers to the degree to which less powerful members of institutions and organizations expect and accept that power is unequally distributed. Higher scores indicate a stronger acceptance of this unequal power distribution. Uncertainty avoidance refers to the degree to which societies avoid uncertainty, are anxious, feel threatened by unknown situations, prefer clear procedures, and respect rules. Higher scores indicate a stronger uncertainty avoidance. Collectivism vs. individualism describes the degree to which collectivism (group cohesion and interests) dominates over individualism (one’s own and close kins’ interests). Lower scores indicate a bias towards collectivism, and higher scores–towards individualism. Masculinity describes the degree to which male vs. female role patterns dominate in a society, with higher scores favouring the male pattern. Long-term vs. short term orientation refers to cultures that make long-term or short-term plans, respectively. The scale considers prudence, thrift, persistence, perseverance, and willingness to sacrifice the present to favour future successes. Higher scores indicate a preference for long-term planning.

Participants expressed their agreement with each statement on a 7-point Likert scale, and the scores were averaged for each subscale (see Table 1).

The World Value Survey 5 [43].

This 6-item questionnaire determines in-group and out-group trust levels on a 4-point Likert scale (see Table 1). For the in-group trust level, participants rate their trust of family members, neighbours, and people they know personally. For out-group trust level, participants rate their trust of people they meet for the first time, from another religion, and another nationality. For the two subscales, the Likert scale scores were averaged (Table 1).

Interpersonal Reactivity Index (IRI [58]; French version from Gilet et al.

[41]). The 28-item IRI questionnaire evaluates the following aspects of empathy: i) feelings of compassion and concern for unfortunate others, ii) the ability to consider the perspective of others, iii) participants’ tendencies to transpose themselves imaginatively or to identify with fictional characters, and iv) self-oriented feelings of personal anxiety and distress in difficult interpersonal situations. Participants rated their agreement with items on a 5-point Likert scale. Of the 28 items, eight were reversely coded. We calculated an average empathy score so that higher scores indicated a natural capacity to share and understand the affective state of others [59] (see Table 1).

Other questionnaires and measures.

In Study 1, we also assessed participants’ self-reported trait autism scores [60] and paranormal belief scores [61]. In both studies, we also asked participants to report on cues they rely on to decide that another person is lying (open question added after the questions on self-reported lying detection abilities, Fig 1D). These data are reported elsewhere.

thumbnail
Fig 1. Flow diagram of events.

The flow diagram depicts parts of the survey that i) were comparable to Study 1 and Study 2 (A, B), ii) were complemented by other questionnaires in Study 2 as compared to Study 1 (C), and iii) used different rating scales for self-reported lie detection abilities (D).

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0285124.g001

Self-reported lie detection abilities.

We asked participants to indicate their self-reported lie detection abilities. In other words, we asked whether they were able to recognize somebody lying. Participants answered on a binary yes/no scale. Afterwards, participants indicated their self-reported lie detection abilities on a 4-point Likert-type scale. They had to rate how accurate they think they were at lie detection, choosing from four options: accurate 0–25% of the time (1), 25–50% (2), 50–75% (3), or 75–100% of the time (4).

Procedure

We used the LimeSurvey platform to prepare and run our online survey. We distributed the online link to potential volunteers. In case of interest, they could complete the survey at their own convenience. On the first two pages, we provided, respectively, written study information and ethical information, such as the right to withdraw from the study at any time, and data confidentiality (Fig 1A). We also stated that we treat participants’ continuation as informed consent. Next, participants provided socio-demographic information regarding their age, gender, nationality, and field of studies (Fig 1B). Then, they completed the self-reported questionnaires in the following order: The FR-C Dark Triad Dirty Dozen, the Cultural Values Scale, the Interpersonal Reactivity Index, and their level of trust based on the World Value Survey 5 (Fig 1C-Study 1 and Table 1). Afterwards, they indicated their self-reported lie detection abilities (Fig 1D-Study 1). Finally, they were fully debriefed and thanked for their participation. The survey took about 20 minutes to complete.

Design and statistical analysis.

To test for the role of individual differences in self-reported lie detection abilities, we considered the self-reported lie detection abilities score as a dependent measure, which was measured on a four-point scale (0–25%; 25–50%; 50–75%; and 75–100%). The histogram showed an uneven distribution. Of the 487 participants, 198 rated their abilities between 25–50% of accuracy and 237 between 50–75%, while only 19 participants self-reported 0–25% of accuracy, and only 33 indicate 75–100% of accuracy. Consequently, we created a dichotomous variable consisting in self-reported lie detection abilities that are below chance level (i.e. scores between 0–25% and 25–50% of accuracy) and above chance level (i.e. scores between 50–75% and 75–100% of accuracy).

To test our study question, we conducted a binary logistic regression analysis on this dependent dichotomous variable (below vs. above chance level) using the following 12 continuous predictor variables: psychopathy, narcissism, Machiavellianism, power distance, uncertainty avoidance, collectivism, masculinity, long-term orientation, empathy, ingroup trust level, outgroup trust level, and gender (see also S1 Table). We presented the correlations between all the predictor variables as supporting information (see S2 Table).

Results

Overall, most participants (81.52%) reported being able to detect lies (yes/no answer) and many participants (55.44%) self-reported that their lie detection abilities ranged between 50% and 100% (i.e. above chance level self-reported lie detection abilities). The likelihood ratio test showed that the overall binary logistic regression model on self-reported lie detection abilities was not significant; LR(12) = 656.23, p =. 360, AIC = 682.23, pseudoR2 = .025 (Cox & Snell), .034 (Nagelkerke) (see S1 Table).

Study 2

Method

Participants.

We used the statistical power analysis tool G*Power [52] to estimate our sample size. We determined a minimum sample size of 231 for a linear multiple regression test with a small effect size of 0.1, 21 predictors, α of 0.05, and power (1−β) of 0.80.

We recruited a new sample of French speaking undergraduate psychology students (N = 700, 90 males) at the University of Lausanne. After excluding incomplete data and matching participants’ age to those of Study 1, we were left with 386 participants (72 males; Mage = 20.21, SDage = 2.22, range = 18 to 31 years). One academic year separated the data collection for Study 1 and Study 2. All participants received course credit for their participation.

Material

Self-report questionnaires. Short Dark Triad (SD3) [62] (French version from Gamache et al. [44]).

This 27-item version measures psychopathy, narcissism and Machiavellianism using a 5-point Likert scale. We used this questionnaire, because the one we had used in Study 1 [54] showed weaker psychometric properties [44, 63].

Big-Six questionnaire (29QB6) [45] (The French translation was done through translation and back translation, the report on the formal validation is currently prepared for publication. In the meantime, the translation can be retrieved here [64]. The 29QB6 assesses personality along six dimensions: i) Extraversion, ii) Conscientiousness, iii) Honesty/Propriety, iv) Resiliency vs. Internalizing Negative Emotionality, v) Agreeableness, and vi) Originality/Talent. Higher Extraversion scores indicate that participants score higher on traits like talkativeness, sociability, assertiveness., gregariousness, and positive emotionality measures. Conscientiousness items refer to being organized, purposeful, and self-controlled. Honesty/Propriety items measure ethical behavior, integrity and deceit, instrumental use of others as well as aspects related to negative valence. Resiliency items capture the ability to internalize negative emotionality as a reverse of Neuroticism. Lower scores on resiliency resemble higher scores on neuroticism [45] Agreeableness items measure kindness and even temper. Originality/Talent items measure perceived talents and abilities, intellectual and aesthetic interest. They also include positive valence content. The 29 items are rated on a 6-point Likert scale, with 14 being reversely coded (see Table 2). Higher scores indicate higher level of expression of each personality trait (see Table 2).

Trait Meta Mood Scale [65] (French version from Maria et al.

[48]). This 30-item questionnaire measures emotional intelligence by assessing the ability to monitor and manage one’s own emotions. This scale consists in three subscales: i) emotional attention–the extent to which individuals attend to and value their feelings; ii) emotional clarity–the extent to which they feel clear about their feelings; and iii) emotional repair–the extent to which they use positive thinking to repair their negative mood. Participants rated each item on a 5-point Likert scale. Higher scores indicated higher levels of emotional attention, emotional clarity, and emotional repair (see Table 2).

Balanced Inventory of Desirable Responding [66] (French version from D’Amours-Raymond [49]).

This 21-item questionnaire measures socially desirable responding along two dimensions: i) self-deceptive positivity–the tendency to give self-reports believed to have a positivity bias, and ii) impression management–deliberate effort at deception. We calculated the total score by summing all the responses on the 7-point Likert scale. We followed authors’ recommendations and coded presence of social desirability (yes) when participants scored between 6 and 7, and absence of social desirability (no) for values below 5 (see Table 2).

Other questionnaires.

The remaining self-reported questionnaires were the same as in Study 1: i) General empathy score using the 28-item Interpersonal Reactivity Index, ii) Cultural dimensions using the 26-item Individual Cultural Values Scale, and iii) trust levels using the World Value Survey 5. All our questionnaires demonstrated good or acceptable internal consistency (see Table 2).

Self-reported lie detection abilities.

We asked participants to indicate if they were able to recognize somebody lying (yes/no answer) and rate their self-reported lie detection abilities on a continuous scale ranging from 0 (unable to detect lies) to 100 (perfectly able to detect lies). Afterwards, we asked participants to report their lie detection abilities in comparison to others from 0 (much worse than others) to 100 (much better than others) (see also [2325, 51]).

Procedure

The procedure was comparable to Study 1 in recruitment and programming, as well as in the sequence of events (see Fig 1). Participants completed the Cultural Values Scale, the Interpersonal Reactivity Index, their level of trust, the Dark Triad, the Balanced Inventory of Desirable Responding, the Trait and Meta Mood Scale, and the Big-Six questionnaires (Fig 1C-Study 2). Afterwards, participants self-reported their lie detection abilities (Fig 1D-Study 2). The entire study took about 20 minutes to complete.

Statistical analysis

We performed two multiple regression analyses to test whether our individual difference measures predicted i) participants’ self-reported lie detection abilities score (0–100), and ii) participants’ self-reported lie detection abilities in comparison to others (see Table 3). We present results of the correlations between all predictor variables in supporting information (see S3 Table).

thumbnail
Table 3. Results of the two multiple regression analysis on the continuous 22 predictor variables for self-reported lie detection abilities and self-reported lie detection abilities in comparison to others.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0285124.t003

Results

Most participants (76.94%) reported being able to detect lies (yes/no answer). Many participants (62.69%) reported that their abilities to detect lies was above chance level (i.e., higher than 50% of accuracy). However, when asked to rate their abilities in comparison to others, only 20.46% of the participants reported being better than others at detecting lies. Most participants (59.59%) estimated their abilities to be below those of others. However, a Pearson correlation on these two self-report measures was significant, r(386) = .683, p < .001 (see S3 Table), higher self-reported lie detection abilities correlated with higher self-reported lie detection abilities in comparison to others.

The overall multiple regression model on self-reported lie detection abilities was significant, F(363, 22) = 2.49, p < .001, Radj2 = .078. Results indicated that the model explained 7.8% of the variance (see Table 3). As shown in Table 3, lower out-group trust levels and higher social desirability predicted higher self-reported lie detection abilities. The remaining predictor variables were not significant (see Table 3).

The multiple linear regression analysis on self-reported lie detection abilities in comparison to others was significant, F(363, 22) = 3.57, p < .001, Radj2 = .128. The model explained 12.8% of the variance (see Table 3). Again, enhanced social desirability significantly predicted enhanced self-reported lie detection abilities in comparison to others (see Table 3). We also found that higher originality and psychopathy predicted higher self-reported lie detection abilities in comparison to others (see Table 3). The remaining predictor variables were not significant (see Table 3).

Discussion

The literature on lie detection demonstrates a little understood paradox. Subjectively, many people self-report being above average in lie detection abilities [21, 2325, 50], while objectively, at least on a group level, people perform around chance level [15, 16]. In two successive online studies, we investigated if and how individual differences can predict self-reported lie detection abilities. In Study 1, we measured Dark Triad personality traits, empathy, cultural values, and trust levels. In Study 2, we additionally accounted for conventional personality traits, emotional intelligence, and social desirability. As outcome measures, participants indicated whether they could detect lies (dichotomous yes/no answer), and to what extent. In Study 1, we coded extent as above versus below chance level, and in Study 2 as scores on a 100-point rating scale. In Study 2, we also asked participants to rate their self-reported lie detection abilities in comparison to others (see also [2325, 51]).

In both studies, over 75% of our participants indicated being able to detect lies. When asked about extent, most participants rated their accuracy above chance level (i.e., being 50% of the time correct). Thus, we replicated that individuals on the group level consider themselves able to detect lies (e.g., [21, 2325, 50]), which is well above the performance one could expect from actual lie detection tasks [15, 16]. When asked to rate their abilities in comparison to others, we found, however, that only about 20% of our participants indicated yes, they judged themselves to be superior to others at detecting lies. For extent, the numbers were comparatively low too; participants indicated being only 40% of the time better than others at detecting lies.

Our primary study goal was to investigate individual differences in self-reported lie detection abilities. Contrary to our expectations, we found few systematic relationships. In Study 1, the overall model included 12 predictor variables and was not significant. In Study 2, the overall model included 22 predictor variables and the model was significant. We observed enhanced self-reported lie detection abilities with i) decreasing out-group trust level scores, and ii) increasing social desirability scores. Neither gender, personality traits (i.e., Dark Triad, Big-Six personality traits), social cognition measures (i.e., empathy and emotional intelligence), nor cultural values predicted self-reported lie detection abilities. Interestingly, our individual difference measures were better predictors of self-reported lie detection abilities in comparison to others. Namely, enhanced psychopathy, enhanced Originality/Talent, and enhanced social desirability predicted enhanced self-reported lie detection abilities in comparison to others. The findings on psychopathy and Originality/Talent (i.e., Openness) have been reported previously [22, 51].

Given our unexpected results, we decided to focus on three major observations. First, very few of our individual difference measures predicted self-reported lie detection abilities. We discuss in detail results on the Dark triad personality traits, because we had the strongest predictions for these traits [2125]. Second, we replicated two previous results, the one on psychopathy [22] and the one Originality/Talent [51]. Yet, these replications were observed when looking at self-reported lie detection abilities in comparison to others. Third, we observed that increased social desirability scores linked to enhanced self-reported lie detection abilities in general as well as in comparison to others.

When designing the studies, we selected individual difference measures that have been mentioned in the lying literature before [20, 22, 23, 25, 33, 34, 37, 39]. Most obvious was the Dark Triad, because of what it stands for, malevolent behavioral tendencies such as self-promotion, manipulation (including lying), and lack of empathy [2125]. Moreover, the Dark Triad had been previously linked to self-reported lie detection abilities [2225]. We found that higher psychopathy scores were associated with higher self-reported lie detection abilities, but only when participants were asked to indicate their abilities in comparison to others. Wissing and Reinhard [22] found a similar relationship, but for higher confidence in self-reported lie detection performances. Thus, we found similar results despite studies using different response modes, suggesting that response mode does not matter. Against this suggestion, we did not replicate that higher levels of narcissism were associated with a higher self-reported lie detection abilities in comparison to others [2325]. Likewise, we did not find that higher levels of narcissism were associated with higher self-reported lie detection abilities.

It is possible that differences between Dark Triad studies are due to differences in populations and measurements. For populations, this possibility is unlikely, because all relevant Dark Triad studies tested undergraduate university students from Western countries. In our case, we tested French speakers, largely in Switzerland, once 487 (99 males) participants and once 386 participants (72 males). Another study tested 207 participants (83 males) from Germany [22], and still other studies tested, respectively, 125 males [23], and 100 undergraduate students (15 males) [25] from Israel. These populations were comparable in gender composition and age, several being biased towards females and others towards males, with the mean population age not exceeding 30 years old.

For measurements, studies differed. We used different Dark Triad questionnaires in Study 1 and Study 2, both being shortened versions [40, 44, 54, 62]. In Study 1, we had used the 12-items Dirty Dozen questionnaire [40, 54], and did not replicate previous findings [2225]. As questionnaire brevity comes with costs, such as reduced construct validity [63], we used the longer and psychometrically superior 27-items Short Dark Triad questionnaire in Study 2 [44, 62]. However, results were comparable for Study 1 and Study 2. One possible explanation could be questionnaire length, because relationships with narcissism scores were found for the 40-item Narcissistic Personality Inventory [2325, 68, 69]. Yet, length cannot explain it all, because relationships between elevated psychopathy scores, overall Dark Triad scores, and confidence in lie detection abilities had been found using an even shorter 9-item scale [22, 70]. As of now, we have insufficient evidence to argue that divergent findings were due to populations or measurements.

Overall, we found hardly any individual difference measures, significantly predicting self-reported lie detection abilities. The situation was not really different when participants rated their lie detection abilities in comparison to others. We found that social desirability scores predicted enhanced ratings of both self-reported lie detection measures. Thus, self-reported lie detection abilities, whether in general or in comparison to others, tap into something that is akin to an attitude or a social value. Maybe, this latter proposition might also explain why lower levels of out-group trust predicted higher self-reported lie detection abilities. In this regard, self-reported lie detection abilities might reflect people’s truth-default state [20] which implies that we assume that others are trustworthy. In addition, indicating that one is able to detect lies reflects on people’s favorable self-view [21, 71, 72]. In other words, it is better for one’s self-esteem to think that one is not easily deceived.

Study limitations, challenges and future directions

We worked on self-reported lie detection abilities without assessing people’s actual lie detection performance. Notwithstanding, on a group level, we assume that our populations’ actual lie detection performances would mirror results of previous studies showing that lie detection performance is around chance level [15, 16]. Future studies should assess both actual and self-reported lie detection abilities in a within-subjects design.

As with most studies in psychology, we tested undergraduate psychology students. They are not representative of the general population, whether within or between cultures (see e.g., [38, 73, 74]. Therefore, the generalizability of our results remains questionable. To encompass this limitation, we are currently running further studies testing alternative populations such as particular groups of professionals who have a relatively enhanced probability encountering deception (i.e., police officers, teacher, and insurers). We will report these results in the future. Moreover, further studies could consider age, because studies showed that lie detection abilities decreased with age [7578] and lies were spotted more easily in older than younger people [78].

Worth noting, we did not replicate that our participants reported being above average in self-reported lie detection abilities, i.e., being in average better when comparing oneself to others (e.g., [21, 2325, 50]). This observation reflects the better-than-average effect [21, 79]. On the contrary, only 20% of our participants thought they were better than others. When asked about extent, only 40% of them indicated being better than others at detecting lies. We could have expected this better-than-average effect, because it seems more pronounced for young people, and for dimensions that lack external verification [72]. These conditions both apply to our study. Another observation, we might have found comparable results with previous studies using self-reported lie detection abilities in comparison to others, if we would have highlighted the middle point of the rating scale, just like the other studied did. Previous studies indicated 50 as the point at which participants rated themselves as good as others [2325]. In contrast, we asked participants to use the scale from 0 (much worse than others) to 100 (much better than others). Therefore, the average is not explicitly presented to the participants.

As a final critical point, some of the self-report questionnaires lacked validity [63, 80, 81]. Returning to the Dark Triad, these self-report questionnaires seemed to extract the uniqueness between narcissism, psychopathy, and Machiavellianism [82]. Several studies, however, reported an overlap between the three constructs and claimed they were not independent [81, 83]. Kajonius et al. [80] even argued that the Dirty Dozen questionnaire assesses only two constructs, namely, narcissism and an anti-social trait (i.e., Machiavellianism and psychopathy combined). Consistent with this, Persson et al. [84] showed that Machiavellianism and psychopathy, measured by the Short Dark Triad, were similar concepts. Therefore, future studies investigating similar questions to ours should include different measures, such as the Narcissistic Personality Inventory [69], because the latter resulted in comparable findings between studies [2325]. Regarding social desirability, several studies emphasized the lack of validity of the BIDR (Balanced Inventory of Desirable Responding) [85, 86]. To address this limitation, further studies are needed to measure social desirability as a trait, using validated measures of related dimensions such as self-control (e.g. [87]).

Conclusion

In two online studies, we tested self-reported lie detection abilities. On the group level, we replicated that people reported being above chance in lie detection. For individual difference measures, we observed that participants had higher lie detection abilities with increasing social desirability and decreasing out-group trust levels, respectively. Unlike our predictions, all other predictor variables were not significant, including the Dark triad and conventional personality traits, empathy, emotional intelligence, and cultural values. When participants rated their lie detection abilities in comparison to others, we did not find the better-than-average effect. Yet, we replicated that enhanced psychopathy and Originality/Talent predicted the latter measure. Our results indicated that it was socially desirable to trust others and to believe being able to detect lies. Future studies should test whether self-reported and actual lie detection abilities are associated and in which way, as well as assess these relationships in different professional groups, different age groups, and in populations beyond the frequently researched Western societies.

Supporting information

S1 Table. Results of the binary logistic regression analysis on the continuous 12 predictor variables for self-reported lie detection abilities (below vs above chance level).

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0285124.s001

(TIF)

S2 Table. Pearson’s correlations between individual characteristics and self-reported lie detection abilities for Study 1.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0285124.s002

(TIF)

S3 Table. Pearson’s correlations between individual characteristics and self-reported lie detection abilities for Study 2.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0285124.s003

(TIF)

References

  1. 1. Taborsky B, Oliveira RF. Social competence: An evolutionary approach. Trends Ecol Evol. 2012 Dec;27(12):679–88. pmid:23040461
  2. 2. Birkhauer J, Gaab J, Kossowsky J, Hasler S, Krummenacher P, Werner C, et al. Trust in the health care professional and health outcome: A meta-analysis. PLoS ONE. 2017 Feb 09;12(2). pmid:28170443
  3. 3. Hungerford C, Cleary M. ‘High trust’ and ‘low trust’ workplace settings: Implications for our mental health and wellbeing. Issues Ment Health Nurs. 2021 Oct 05;42(5):506–514. pmid:33016852
  4. 4. Marková I, Linell P, Gillespie A. Trust and distrust in society, trust and distrust: Sociocultural perspectives. Charlotte (NC): Information Age Publishing; 2008.
  5. 5. Delmas H, Elissalde B, Rochat N, Demarchi S, Tijus C, Urdapilleta I. Policemen’s and civilians’ beliefs about facial cues of deception. J Nonverbal Behav. 2019 Oct 08;43(1):59–90.
  6. 6. Serota KB, Levine TR. A few prolific liars: Variation in the prevalence of lying. J Lang Soc Psychol. 2014 Apr 04;34(2):138–57.
  7. 7. DePaulo BM, Kashy DA, Kirkendol SE, Wyer MM, Epstein JA. Lying in everyday life. J Pers Soc Psychol. 1996 May;70:979–95. pmid:8656340
  8. 8. DePaulo BM, Kashy DA. Everyday lies in close and casual relationships. J Pers Soc Psychol. 1998 Jan;74:63–79. pmid:9457776
  9. 9. Levine TR, Ali MV, Dean M, Abdulla RA, Garcia-Ruano K. Toward a pan-cultural typology of deception motives. J Intercult Commun Res. 2016 Feb 01;45(1):1–12.
  10. 10. Daiku Y, Serota KB, Levine TR. A few prolific liars in Japan: Replication and the effects of Dark Triad personality traits. PLoS ONE. 2021 Apr 15;16(4):e0249815. pmid:33857197
  11. 11. Halevy R, Shalvi S, Verschuere B. Being honest about dishonesty: Correlating self-reports and actual lying. Hum Commun Res. 2014 Jan 01;40(1):54–72.
  12. 12. Camden C, Motley MT, Wilson A. White lies in interpersonal communication: A taxonomy and preliminary investigation of social motivations. West J Speech Commun. 1984 Jun 06;48(4):309–25.
  13. 13. Ekman P O’Sullivan M. Who can catch a liar? Am Psychol. 1991 Sep;46(9):913–20. pmid:1958011
  14. 14. Levine TR, Kim RK, Hamel LM. People lie for a reason: Three experiments documenting the principle of veracity. Commun Res Rep. 2010 Oct 29;27(4):271–85.
  15. 15. Bond CF, DePaulo BM. Accuracy of deception judgments. Pers Soc Psychol Rev. 2006 Aug;10(3):214–34. pmid:16859438
  16. 16. Bond CF, DePaulo BM. Individual differences in judging deception: Accuracy and bias. Psychol Bull. 2008 Jul;134(4):477–92. pmid:18605814
  17. 17. Elaad E. Effects of feedback on the overestimated capacity to detect lies and the underestimated ability to tell lies. Appl Cogn Psychol. 2003 Feb 26;17(3):349–63.
  18. 18. Elaad E. Lie-detection biases among male police interrogators, prisoners, and laypersons. Psychol Rep. 2009 Dec 01;105(3 Pt 2):1047–56. pmid:20229908
  19. 19. Clare DD, Levine TR. Documenting the Truth-Default: The low frequency of spontaneous unprompted veracity assessments in deception detection. Hum Commun Res. 2019 Feb 16;45(3):286–308.
  20. 20. Levine TR. Truth-Default Theory (TDT): A theory of human deception and deception detection. J Lang Soc Psychol. 2014 May 23;33(4):378–92.
  21. 21. Personality Elaad E., demographic, and psychophysiological correlates of people’s self-assessed lying abilities. In: Rosenfeld P. Detecting concealed information and deception: Recent developments. Elsevier ed. Evanston: Academic press; 2018. pp. 353–574.
  22. 22. Wissing BG, Reinhard MA. The Dark Triad and the PID-5 Maladaptive Personality Traits: Accuracy, confidence and response bias in judgments of veracity. Front Psychol. 2017 Sep 21;8:1549. pmid:28983264
  23. 23. Zvi L, Elaad E. Correlates of narcissism, self-reported lies, and self-assessed abilities to tell and detect lies, tell truths, and believe others. J. Investig. Psychol. Offender Profiling. 2018 Sep 12;15(3):271–86.
  24. 24. Elaad E, Hanania SB, Mazor S, Zvi L. The relations between deception, narcissism and self-assessed lie- and truth-related abilities. Psychiatr Psychol Law. 2020 May 5;27(5):880–93. pmid:33833615
  25. 25. Elaad E. Deceptive behavior: Effects of rational thinking, narcissism, and self-assessed lie- and truth related abilities. SAGE Open. 2022 Apr;12(2):21582440221085012.
  26. 26. Elaad E, Gonen-Gal Y. Face-to-face lying: Gender and motivation to deceive. Front Psychol. 2022 Mar 22;13:820923. pmid:35391990
  27. 27. Capraro V. Gender differences in lying in sender-receiver games: A meta-analysis. Judgm Decis Mak. 2018 Jul;13(4):345–55.
  28. 28. Dreber A, Johannesson M. Gender differences in deception. Econ Lett. 2008 Apr;99(1):197–9.
  29. 29. Gylfason HF, Arnardottir AA, Kristinsson K. More on gender differences in lying. Econ Lett. 2013 Apr;119(1):94–6.
  30. 30. Duran G, Cécillon FX, Sansorgné T, Michael GA. Cold-blooded women can detect lies with greater accuracy than other women. J Forens Psychiatry Psychol. 2019 Dec 26;30(3):510–29.
  31. 31. Neves L, Cordeiro C, Scott SK, Castro SL, Lima CF. High emotional contagion and empathy are associated with enhanced detection of emotional authenticity in laughter. Q J Exp Psychol. 2018 Jan 01;71(11):2355–63. pmid:30362411
  32. 32. Williams DM, Nicholson T, Grainger C, Lind SE, Carruthers P. Can you spot a liar? Deception, mindreading, and the case of autism spectrum disorder. Autism Res. 2018 Apr 27;11(8):1129–37. pmid:29701910
  33. 33. Peace KA, Sinclair SM. Cold-blooded lie catchers? An investigation of psychopathy, emotional processing, and deception detection. Legal Criminol Psychol. 2011 Mar 18;17(1):177–91.
  34. 34. Zloteanu M, Bull P, Krumhuber EG, Richardson DC. Veracity judgement, not accuracy: Reconsidering the role of facial expressions, empathy, and emotion recognition training on deception detection. Q J Exp Psychol. 2020 Nov 25;74(5):910–27. pmid:33234008
  35. 35. Fiori M. A new look at emotional intelligence: A dual-process framework. Pers Soc Psychol Rev. 2009 Feb 01;13(1):21–44. pmid:19114503
  36. 36. O’Sullivan M. Emotional intelligence and deception detection: why most people can’t "read" others, but a few can. In: Riggio RE, editor. Applications of nonverbal communication. Mahwah (NJ): Lawrence Erlbaum Associates Publishers; 2005. pp. 215–253.
  37. 37. Baker A, ten Brinke L, Porter S. Will get fooled again: Emotionally intelligent people are easily duped by high-stakes deceivers. Legal Criminol Psychol. 2013 Sep;18(2):300–13.
  38. 38. Algan Y, Cahuc P. Trust and growth. Annu Rev Econom. 2013 May 17;5(1):521–49.
  39. 39. Triandis HC, Carnevale P, Gelfand M, Robert C, Wasti SA, Probst T, et al. Culture and deception in business negotiations: A multilevel analysis. J. Cross Cult. Manag. 2001 Apr;1(1):73–90.
  40. 40. Savard C, Simard C, Jonason PK. Psychometric properties of the French-Canadian version of the Dark Triad Dirty Dozen. Pers Individ Dif. 2017 Dec 1;119:122–8.
  41. 41. Gilet AL, Mella N, Studer J, Grühn D, Labouvie-Vief G. Assessing dispositional empathy in adults: A French validation of the Interpersonal Reactivity Index (IRI). Can J Behav Sci. 2013 Jan;45(1):42–48.
  42. 42. Zheng L. Antecedents and consequences of perceived risk in Internet shopping in China and France: a cross-cultural approach/Les antécédents et les conséquences des risques perçus dans les achats sur Internet en Chine et en France: une approche interculturelle. Doctoral Thesis, Université de Grenoble. 2013.317 p.
  43. 43. Inglehart R, Haerpfer C, Moreno A, Welzel C, Kizilova K, Diez-Medrano J, et al. World values survey: Round six-country-pooled datafile version. Madrid: JD systems institute. 2014;12.
  44. 44. Gamache D, Savard C, Maheux-Caron V. French adaptation of the Short Dark Triad: Psychometric properties and a head-to-head comparison with the Dirty Dozen. Pers Individ Dif. 2018 Feb 01;122:164–70.
  45. 45. Thalmayer AG, Saucier G, Eigenhuis A. Comparative validity of brief to medium-length Big Five and Big Six Personality Questionnaires. Psychol Assess. 2011 Dec;23(4):995–1009. pmid:21859221
  46. 46. Jonason PK, McCain J. Using the HEXACO model to test the validity of the Dirty Dozen measure of the Dark Triad. Pers Individ Dif. 2012 Nov;53(7):935–38.
  47. 47. Stead R, Fekken GC. Agreeableness at the core of the dark triad of personality. Individ Differ Res. 2014 Dec;12(4-A):131–41.
  48. 48. Maria AS, Bourdier LM, Duclos JP, Ringuenet DM, Berthoz SP. Propriétés psychométriques de la version française d’une échelle de mesure de l’intelligence émotionnelle perçue: la Trait Meta-Mood Scale (TMMS)/Psychometric properties of the French version of a scale measuring perceived emotional intelligence: the Trait Meta-Mood Scale (TMMS). Can J Psychiatry. 2016 Oct;61(10):652–62.
  49. 49. D’Amours-Raymond J. Version abrégée transculturelle du Balanced Inventory of Desirable Responding (BIDR). Master Thesis, The University of Laval. 2011. 66p.
  50. 50. Elaad E, Lavy S, Cohenca D, Berholz E, Thee P, Ben-Gigi Y. Lies, truths, and attachment orientations in late adolescence. Pers Individ Dif. 2012 Apr;52(6):670–3.
  51. 51. Elaad E, Reizer A. Personality correlates of the self-assessed abilities to tell and detect lies, tell truths, and believe others. J Individ Differ. 2015 Jan;36(3):163–69.
  52. 52. Faul F, Erdfelder E, Buchner A, Lang AG. Statistical power analyses using G*Power 3.1: Tests for correlation and regression analyses. Behav Res Methods. 2009 Nov;41(4):1149–60. pmid:19897823
  53. 53. Chen H, Cohen P, Chen S. How big is a big odds ratio? Interpreting the magnitudes of odds ratios in epidemiological studies. Commun Stat Simul Comput. 2010 Apr; 39(4):860–4.
  54. 54. Jonason PK, Webster GD. The dirty dozen: A concise measure of the dark triad. Psychol Assess. 2010 Jun;22(2):420–32. pmid:20528068
  55. 55. Yoo B, Donthu N, Lenartowicz T. Measuring Hofstede’s Five dimensions of cultural values at the individual level: Development and validation of CVSCALE. J Int Consum Mark. 2011 Jun 24;23(3–4):193–210.
  56. 56. Hofstede G. Cultural dimensions in management and planning. Asia Pac. J. Manag. 1984 Jan;1(2):81–99.
  57. 57. Hofstede G. Dimensionalizing cultures: The Hofstede Model in context. Online readings in psychology and culture. 2011 Dec 1;2(1).
  58. 58. Davis MH. A multidimensional approach to individual differences in empathy. Catalog of selected documents in psychology. 1980;10:1–17.
  59. 59. Decety J, Lewis KL, Cowell JM. Specific electrophysiological components disentangle affective sharing and empathic concern in psychopathy. J Neurophysiol. 2015 Jul 17;114(1):493–504. pmid:25948868
  60. 60. Allison C, Auyeung B, Baron-Cohen S. Toward brief “red flags” for Autism screening: The Short Autism Spectrum Quotient and the Short Quantitative Checklist in 1,000 cases and 3,000 controls. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry. 2012 Feb;51(2):202–12. pmid:22265366
  61. 61. Tobayck J. A Revised Paranormal Belief Scale. International Journal of Transpersonal Studies. 2004;23:94–98.
  62. 62. Jones DN, Paulhus DL. Introducing the Short Dark Triad (SD3): A brief measure of dark personality traits. Assessment. 2014 Feb;21(1):28–41. pmid:24322012
  63. 63. Miller JD, Few LR, Seibert LA, Watts A, Zeichner A, Lynam DR. An examination of the Dirty Dozen measure of psychopathy: A cautionary tale about the costs of brief measures. Psychol Assess. 2012 Dec;24(4):1048–53. pmid:22612650
  64. 64. Fernandes M, Udayar S, Jonauskaite D, Rossier J, Mohr C, Thalmayer AG. French version of the Big-Six inventory (29 items); 2023 [cited 2023 Mar 28]. Database: OSF[Internet]. Available from: https://osf.io/btn59/.
  65. 65. Salovey P, Mayer JD, Goldman SL, Turvey C, Palfai T. Emotional attention, clarity, and repair: Exploring emotional intelligence using the Trait Meta-Mood Scale. In: Pennebaker JW, editor. Emotion, disclosure, & healt. Washington (DC): Am Psychol; 1995. p. 125–54.
  66. 66. Paulhus DL. Measurement and control of response bias. In: Robinson JP, Shaver PR, editors.Measures of personality and social psychological attitudes. San Diego (CA): Academic Press; 1991. p. 17–59.
  67. 67. Delhey J, Welzel C. Generalizing trust: How outgroup-trust grows beyond ingroup-trust. WVR. 2012 Feb 05;5(3).
  68. 68. Raskin R, Terry H. A principal-components analysis of the Narcissistic Personality Inventory and further evidence of its construct validity. J Pers Soc Psychol. 1988 May;54(5):890–902. pmid:3379585
  69. 69. Raskin RN, Hall CS. A narcissistic personality inventory. Psychol Rep. 1979 Oct;45(2):590. pmid:538183
  70. 70. Kuefner AC, Dufner M, Back MD. The Dirty Dozen and the Naughty Nine: Short scales for the assessment of narcissism, Machiavellianism, and psychopathy. Diagnostica. 2015 Apr 22;61(2):76–91.
  71. 71. Kruger J, Dunning D. Unskilled and unaware of it: How difficulties in recognizing one’s own incompetence lead to inflated self-assessments. Am Psychol; 1999. p. 1121–34. pmid:10626367
  72. 72. Zell E, Strickhouser JE, Sedikides C, Alicke MD. The better-than-average effect in comparative self-evaluation: A comprehensive review and meta-analysis. Am Psychol. 2020. p. 118–49. pmid:31789535
  73. 73. Taylor PJ, Larner S, Conchie SM, Menacere T. Culture moderates changes in linguistic self-presentation and detail provision when deceiving others. R Soc Open Sci. 2017 Jun;4(6):170128. pmid:28680668
  74. 74. Taylor PJ, Larner S, Conchie SM, van der Zee S. Cross-cultural deception detection. In Granhag PA, Vrij A, Verschuere B. Detecting deception: Current challenges and cognitive approaches. John Wiley & Sons; 2014. pp. 175– 201.
  75. 75. Castle E, Eisenberger NI, Seeman TE, Moons WG, Boggero IA, Grinblatt MS, et al. Neural and behavioral bases of age differences in perceptions of trust. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2012 Oct 24;109(51):20848–52. pmid:23213232
  76. 76. O’Connor AM, Lyon TD, Evans AD. Younger and older adults’ lie-detection and credibility judgments of children’s coached reports. Psychol Crime Law. 2019 Apr 05;25(9):925–44. pmid:31988596
  77. 77. Poulin MJ, Haase CM. Growing to trust: Evidence that trust increases and sustains well-being across the life span. Soc Psychol Personal Sci. 2015 Mar 02;6(6):614–21.
  78. 78. Ruffman T, Murray J, Halberstadt J, Vater T. Age-related differences in deception. Psychol Aging. 2012 Sep;27(3): 543–9. pmid:21463058
  79. 79. Alicke MD, Govorun O. The Better-Than-Average Effect. The Self in Social Judgment: Psychology Press; 2005. p. 85–106.
  80. 80. Kajonius PJ, Persson BN, Rosenberg P, Garcia D. The (mis)measurement of the Dark Triad Dirty Dozen: exploitation at the core of the scale. PeerJ. 2016 Mar 1;4: e1748. pmid:26966673
  81. 81. Muris P, Merckelbach H, Otgaar H, Meijer E. The Malevolent side of human nature: A meta-analysis and critical review of the literature on the Dark Triad (narcissism, Machiavellianism, and psychopathy). Perspect Psychol Sci. 2017 Mar 27;12(2):183–204. pmid:28346115
  82. 82. Hodson G, Book A, Visser BA, Volk AA, Ashton MC, Lee K. Is the Dark Triad common factor distinct from low Honesty-Humility? J Res Pers. 2018 Apr;73:123–29.
  83. 83. Furnham A, Richards SC, Paulhus DL. The Dark Triad of personality: A 10 Year review. Soc Personal Psychol Compass. 2013 Mar 05;7(3):199–216.
  84. 84. Persson BN, Kajonius PJ, Garcia D. Revisiting the structure of the Short Dark Triad. Assessment. 2019 Jan;26(1):3–16. pmid:28382846
  85. 85. Holden RR, Passey J. Socially desirable responding in personality assessment: Not necessarily faking and not necessarily substance. Pers Individ Dif. 2010 Oct;49(5):446–50.
  86. 86. Lanz L, Thielmann I, Gerpott FH. Are social desirability scales desirable? A meta-analytic test of the validity of social desirability scales in the context of prosocial behavior. J Pers. 2022 Apr;90(2):203–21. pmid:34265863
  87. 87. Tangney JP, Baumeister RF, Boone AL. High self-control predicts good adjustment, less pathology, better grades, and interpersonal success. J Pers. 2004 Apr;72(2):271–324. pmid:15016066