Skip to main content
Advertisement
  • Loading metrics

Tropilaelaps mercedesae parasitism changes behavior and gene expression in honey bee workers

  • Jing Gao ,

    Roles Formal analysis, Methodology, Validation, Writing – original draft, Writing – review & editing

    ‡ These authors contributed equally to this work and should be considered co-first authors.

    Affiliation Key Laboratory of Pollinating Insect Biology, Institute of Apicultural Research, Chinese Academy of Agricultural Sciences, Beijing, China

  • Shilong Ma ,

    Roles Formal analysis, Methodology, Writing – original draft

    ‡ These authors contributed equally to this work and should be considered co-first authors.

    Affiliations Key Laboratory of Pollinating Insect Biology, Institute of Apicultural Research, Chinese Academy of Agricultural Sciences, Beijing, China, College of Bee Science, Fujian Agriculture and Forestry University, Fuzhou, China

  • Xinling Wang,

    Roles Methodology

    Affiliation Key Laboratory of Pollinating Insect Biology, Institute of Apicultural Research, Chinese Academy of Agricultural Sciences, Beijing, China

  • Yang Yang,

    Roles Methodology

    Affiliation Key Laboratory of Pollinating Insect Biology, Institute of Apicultural Research, Chinese Academy of Agricultural Sciences, Beijing, China

  • Qihua Luo,

    Roles Investigation, Methodology

    Affiliation Miyun Apicultural Station, Beijing, China

  • Xing Wang,

    Roles Investigation, Methodology

    Affiliation Beijing Apicultural Station, Beijing, China

  • Feng Liu,

    Roles Investigation, Methodology

    Affiliation Jiangxi Institute of Apicultural Research, Nanchang, China

  • Qiang Wang,

    Roles Investigation

    Affiliation Key Laboratory of Pollinating Insect Biology, Institute of Apicultural Research, Chinese Academy of Agricultural Sciences, Beijing, China

  • Zhongmin Fu,

    Roles Investigation

    Affiliation College of Bee Science, Fujian Agriculture and Forestry University, Fuzhou, China

  • Qingyun Diao,

    Roles Funding acquisition

    Affiliation Key Laboratory of Pollinating Insect Biology, Institute of Apicultural Research, Chinese Academy of Agricultural Sciences, Beijing, China

  • Pingli Dai

    Roles Conceptualization, Formal analysis, Funding acquisition, Methodology, Validation, Writing – original draft, Writing – review & editing

    daipingli@caas.cn

    Affiliation Key Laboratory of Pollinating Insect Biology, Institute of Apicultural Research, Chinese Academy of Agricultural Sciences, Beijing, China

Abstract

Tropilaelaps mercedesae is one of the most problematic honey bee parasites and has become more threatening to the beekeeping industry. Tropilaelaps can easily parasitize immature honey bees (larvae and pupae) and have both lethal and sublethal effects on the individual worker bees. Our study for the first time experimentally assessed the effects of T. mercedesae on olfactory learning, flight ability, homing ability as well as transcriptional changes in parasitized adult honey bees. T. mercedesae infestation had negative impacts on olfactory associated function, flight ability, and homing rate. The volume of the mushroom body significantly increased in infested honey bees, which may be correlated to the lower sucrose responsiveness as well as lower learning ability in the infested bees. The gene expression involved in immune systems and carbohydrate transport and metabolism were significantly different between infested bees and non-infested bees. Moreover, genes function in cell adhesion play an essential role in olfactory sensory in honey bees. Our findings provide a comprehensive understanding of European honey bees in response to T. mercedesae infestation, and could be used to further investigate the complex molecular mechanisms in honey bees under parasitic stress.

Author summary

In recent decades, there has been serious concern about the decline of honey bees in the world. One of the most serious factors contributing to bee population declines is mite parasitism. Although Varroa destructor is the most widespread globally, Tropilaelaps mercedesae displays greater threat to bee colonies due to its smaller size, shorter phoretic phase, more rapid locomotion, as well as faster reproductive rate. Tropilaelaps mites, originally parasite of the giant Asian honey bees, now becoming an emerging threat of European honey bees (Apis mellifera) in Asian area. This work aimed to investigate the influence of T. mercedesae infestation on behavior and gene expression in A. mellifera. Our results highlight the T. mercedesae infestation induced negative effects of olfactory learning, flight ability, homing ability of honey bee workers. Moreover, we found that T. mercedesae infestation caused the up-regulation of genes involved in immune systems and carbohydrate mechanism which were correlated to the different olfactory learning performance in infested honeybee. In addition, genes function in cell adhesion play an essential role in olfactory sensory in honey bees. Our results increase the knowledge of proximate mechanisms in honey bee responding to parasitic stress.

Introduction

Pollinators, especially genus Apis, are critical for the production of agricultural crops and maintaining ecosystems. In recent decades, honeybees have consistently declined in population and biodiversity, posing a potential threat to the existence of species and global food security [1]. One of the most critical factors contributing to bee population declines is parasite infestation, which has caused thousands of colony losses worldwide [2]. Previous studies have demonstrated that parasitic mites in the genus Varroa and Tropilaelaps are the major factors causing the collapse of European honey bee (Apis mellifera) in Asian area [36].

Tropilaelaps mites are originally parasites of giant Asian honeybees, and divided into four species (T. clareae, T. koenigerum, T. mercedesae and T. thaii) according to genetic and morphological variation [7]. Over the past few decades, scientists mainly focused on the Varroa mite as it is the worst bee pest worldwide, while the Tropilaelaps mites are only found in Asia. It has been suggested that T. clareae and T. mercedesae have successfully transferred to A. mellifera, whereas T. mercedesa has a wider distribution than T. clareae [6]. Compared with Varroa, Tropilaelaps mites have smaller size, shorter phoretic phase, more rapid locomotion, as well as faster reproductive rate [810]. With these characteristics, Tropilaelaps mites present a greater threat than the infamous Varroa destructor to A. mellifera. Until now, there is no report concerning the Tropilaelaps mites found in European apiculture industry. With the frequent transfer of commercial bee colonies, the bee trade globalization, the natural proliferation of bees, and the gradual warming of the global climate, it may be only a matter of time until Tropilaelaps spread outside of Asia to cause devastating effects on apiculture industry in Europe and North America [6].

T. mercedesae infestation threatens to the health of A. mellifera in many aspects. Similar to Varroa, Tropilaelaps mite can reproduce in the drone and worker brood cells of A. mellifera, but exclusively feed on brood due to the morphology of their mouthparts and body shape [11]. The life span of T. mercedesae on pupae and larvae is longer than that on adult bees, indeed, the mites can only survive for two or three days when there are only adult bees around [5,12]. This characteristic makes their dispersal ability not as strong as the Varroa mites, as they can only disperse on adult bees. Previous studies have indicated that the multiple small wounds derived from feeding of T. mercedesae cause irregular brood patterns, malformed honey bees that are wingless, and dead larvae [6,9]. Like Varroa mites, Tropilaelaps are potential vectors of honey bee viruses, particularly Deformed Wing Virus (DWV) [13]. Honey bee infested by Tropilaelaps during early development stage may enhance viral proliferation in colony, as the longer exposure to the virus and the stress on susceptibility to viral infection [14]. de Guzman et al. has demonstrated that bee pupae infested with either Varroa or Tropilaelaps had higher levels of both DWV variants than uninfested pupae [15]. In addition, feeding by Tropilaelaps mites can induce cellular immune response in worker broods, which may be caused by the injury from mite feeding, viruses infection, or the interactions of both factors [16]. Although lacking a phoretic phase involving honey bee adults, the adverse effects of Tropilaelaps infestation during the precapping stages may last to the adult stage, with remarkably higher numbers of wounds and virus infection rates observed in infested adult honey bees [17]. Infestation of T. mercedesae lead to a reduction in weight and longevity, and increase of the sugar syrup and pollen consumption in adult worker bees [4,18]. The combination of T. mercedesae infestation and imidacloprid 50 μg/L exposure reduced survival and increased pollen consumption of A. mellifera [19].

Despite the various lethal and sublethal effects of T. mercedesae on A. mellifera, information about the effects of T. mercedesae infestation on behavior, organization and molecular mechanisms in individual honey bees is rare [9]. It has been demonstrated that Varroa infestation changed the in-hive behavior of adult honeybees during the pupal stage, such as less involved in tending larvae and dealing with hive duties. Varroa-infested foraging bees and drones show reduced flight and homing ability [2022]. Moreover, Varroa parasitization has a negative impact on the cognitive abilities of adult bees, which may be associated with DWV boosted by V. destructor infestation [23,24]. In addition to its effects on behavior performance, Varroa infection also causes changes in gene expression related to energetic metabolism and immune response [25]. Transcript levels of pathogen recognition gene Peptidoglycan Recognition Proteins (PGRPs) and Prophenoloxidases (PPOs), key enzymes of the melanization reaction and defense mechanism of insects, increased in honey bee larvae exposed to Varroa mites [26]. Comparing the transcriptome changes between A. mellifera and A. cerana inducing by Varroa also shows that the transcriptional expression changes of components responsible for neuronal rewiring, olfaction, metabolism and social behavior may be the key components driving Varroa tolerance [27]. Whether the the negative effects of Varroa on honey bees can be inferred to T. mercedesae keeps unknown.

In this study, we comprehensively investigated the effects of T. mercedesae infestation on a series of behaviors performance, including sucrose responsiveness, olfactory learning, flight and homing ability. Here we specifically tested the hypothesis that the T. mercedesae infestation was correlated with the sensory perception of smell and olfactory learning and memory in the honey bees which can be reflected by changes in the expression of specific genes. This work will contribute to a better understanding of alterations in the physiological and molecular traits in response to T. mercedesae infestation in adult honey bees.

Materials and methods

Honey bees and mites

A. mellifera and T. mercedesae were obtained from an apiary at the Institute of Apicultural Research (40°00’28”N, 116°12’18”E), Chinese Academy of Agricultural Sciences (Beijing, China), during June to October 2018. In the present study, we used ten healthy and strong colonies with no history of bee diseases, five to the T. mercedesae treatment and five to the reproduction of the T. mercedesae. Each colony consisted of six or seven frames of adult bees and two or three frames of brood, which are established in a standard 10-frame Langstroth hive. Since T. mercedesae can only survive for two or three days when there are only adult bees around, we caged queens for one month and removed all brood from the experimental colonies before the experiments to obtain healthy and mite-free colonies. In each colony, broods at the same age were obtained by caging the queen on an empty frame for 24 h to lay eggs. The comb with eggs was divided into small pieces and fixed on a small frame to constitute special comb, and then put back into the original colony [19].

The reproduction of the T. mercedesae in A. mellifera colonies was according to the method reported by Khongphinitbunjong et al [28]. In short, one foundress of Tropilaelaps was introduced into a brood cell with a 5th larval instar after sealing, and mites were inoculated into newly sealed brood cells [19]. All inoculum mites were randomly obtained from highly infested A. mellifera colonies. Other combs with open and closed brood cells without mites were used as controls.

A total of 10 brood combs, five infested combs and five controls were placed in an incubator (34 ± 1°C, 60 ± 10% relative humidity, and darkness). To avoid contamination of the control bees by the infested frame, the control and infested brood frames were incubated in the same incubator but in different layers. Newly emerged bees that infested or non-infested T. mercedesae were collected every 2 h and randomly placed in cages (9×9×10 cm) with 30 bees as a group. The bees were supplied with sufficient syrup (50% w/v sucrose solution) and fresh pollen, and the cages were maintained in the dark in an incubator (30 ± 1°C, RH 60 ± 10%) [29].

Flight ability

The flight ability of infested and non-infested worker bees was tested at 15th day post emergence by using a modified flight mill [30]. This procedure was performed according to the flight procedure reported by Tosi et al.[31]. On each trial, fifteen honey bees were captured from each comb as one replicate. Honey bees were attached to the wire flight mill arm through a 1-cm-long hollow Teflon tube (S1 Fig). Once the honey bees were attached to the flight mill, a computer connected to the flight mill sensor could record mean velocity, flight duration, and flight distance. The experiment was carried out in three replicates and 45 bees for each replicate were employed with 24 h test duration.

Homing ability

Every 2 h, 300 infested and non-infested bees were randomly collected from brood combs of three colonies, respectively. Bees were marked with different color painted on their thorax and placed into three non-infested colonies. To familiarize the tested bees with the experimental background, the experiment started three days after collection from the colonies and introduction into the new colony. Then bees were placed in an incubator (26°C± 1°C, 60 ± 10% relative humidity, darkness) for 2 h. All bees were then released from approximate 50 m away from the hive and each bee was only released once. The homing time and the number of honey bees successfully returning to the hive were recorded.

Sucrose responsiveness and olfactory learning behavior

Sucrose responsiveness was tested according to the standard method for honey bees described in Decourtye et al with with minor changes [32]. Honey bees for proboscis extension reflex (PER) were secured individually in 1.5 mL tubes with their antennae and mouthparts free (S2 Fig). Before feeding assay, tested bees were starved for 4 h, and then keep them in the incubator between testing. Infested and non-infested bees were tested for their response to 30% (w/w) sucrose solutions at 0 (emergence within 24 hours), 5, 10 and 15 days after emergence.

Sucrose stimulation was performed with a soaked toothpick touching both of the bee’s antennae at the same time for 1 s, and the PER was recorded (1 if a bee extended her proboscis and 0 if she did not respond). Alternated water trials between each sugar solution were used to reduce the possible effect of sensory sensitization to antennal touch. All stimulations were performed at 3 min intervals. To avoid invalid counts caused by bees who are only thirsty or not responding to sucrose, bees that responded to water or did not respond to any test concentrations of sugar solution were discarded before the test. The test bees were put back to the incubator after tests were completed. The number of bees died during the experiment was very small and was not statistically analyzed.

Honey bees showing PER after stimulated with a 30% sucrose solution at 15 day after emergence were selected for the olfactory learning behavior test according to Strube-Bloss et al [33]. In short, Linalool (Sigma, 97% purity) acted as the conditioned stimulus through airflow system. In order to familiarize with the mechanical stimulation and with the experimental background, bees were placed in the airflow system with main airflow speed of 50 ml ∙ s-1 added to a secondary airflow speed of 2.5 ml ∙ s-1. The Linalool was soaked on a filter paper strip inserted in a Pasteur pipette cartridge and then delivered through the secondary flow (2.5 ml ∙ s-1) for 6 s. Bees were starved for 4 h prior to conditioning. Three conditioning sessions for individuals were performed at 20 min intervals (conditioning phases C1, C2 and C3). The individuals were then subjected to five test trials (called T1–T5), during which the conditioned stimulus was delivered for 6 s at 20 min intervals without an unconditioned stimulus or a reward with a sucrose solution. The conditioned PER was recorded as a yes-or-no response during the test trials. In each experimental group, 90 bees were conditioned. The experiments were replicated at least three times. After the tests, the bees with different performance were separated and frozen with liquid nitrogen for RNA-seq analysis.

Scanning electron microscopy of the antennae

Infected and non-infected worker bees were randomly collected from each cage on 0, 5, 10 and 15 day after emergence. The 4th, 7th, and 8th flagella of the right antennae were observed according to the method reported by Letzkus et al. with scanning electron microscopy [34].

Paraffin-embedded brain tissue sections

Paraffin-embedded tissue sections of the brains were made according to a previously described method [35]. The test bees were randomly selected from different control and infested colonies. Brain tissue of honey bees at 15th day after emergence were dissected from the head capsule and fixed with 2.5% glutaral. The hematoxylin- and eosin-stained ultrathin sections were observed with transmission electron microscopy.

Experiment design for RNA-seq analysis

We present an experiment designed to test the molecular effects of T. mercedesae Infestation on the irregular performance associated with olfactory function observed in the behavior experiment described above. We collected heads from parasitized or non-parasitized bees with different sucrose responsiveness and olfactory learning behavior for RNA-seq sequencing: (1) non-mite-infested honey bees non-extended the proboscis with 30% sucrose stimulus (CKSN); (2) honey bees infested with T. mercedesae and non-extended the proboscis with 30% sucrose stimulus (TSN); (3) non-mite-infested honey bees non-extended the proboscis with odour stimulus in the first test trial (CKN); (4) honey bees infested with T. mercedesae and non-extended the proboscis by touching the antennae with odour stimulus in the first test trial (TN); (5) non-mite-infested honey bees extended the proboscis during five test trial (CKL); (6) honey bees infested with T. mercedesae and extended the proboscis during five test trial (TL). Samples were immediately frozen in liquid nitrogen and stored at -80°C until RNA extraction.

RNA extraction and RNA sequencing assay

The heads of frozen bees were removed by using a scalpel. Samples were frozen and stored at −80°C until the time of RNA isolation. Total RNA of five bee heads were pooled in each replicate and isolated using TRIzol reagent following the manufacturer’s instruction. For each experimental group, three biological replicates were isolated. A total amount of 3 μg RNA per sample was used as input material for the RNA sample preparations. Total RNA of each sample was isolated using a Quick RNA isolation kit (Bioteke Corporation, Beijing, China) and then assessed using the RNA Nano 6000 Assay Kit of the Agilent Bioanalyzer 2100 system (Agilent Technologies, CA, USA). The construction of the libraries and the RNA-Seq were performed by the Biomarker Biotechnology Corporation (Beijing, China). The mRNA-Seq libraries were generated using the RNA Library Prep Kit (Illumina Inc., San Diego, CA, USA) following standard Illumina protocols. Second strand cDNA synthesis was subsequently performed using DNA Polymerase I and RNase H. cDNAs were used for PCR amplication. PCR products were purified with AMPure XP beads. The cDNA library was quality assessed on the Agilent Bioanalyzer 2100 system. The mRNA-Seq library was performed on the platform (Illumina Inc., San Diego, CA, USA) following the standard Illumina preparation protocol.

Bioinformatics analysis of RNA-seq data

Trimmomatic software (Bolger et al., 2014) was used to remove adaptor sequences, empty reads, short reads (<50 bp), reads with an N-ratio >10%, and low-quality regions [36]. Then clean reads from each sample were mapped to the A. mellifera genome by Hisat2 tools software with a maximum allowance of 2 nucleotide mismatches. The abundance of unigenes was performed by the fragments per kilobase of transcript per million fragments mapped (FPKM) method [37]. Differentially expressed genes (DEGs) of two groups were implemented by the DESeq2 R package (1.18.0) with the threshold of │log2 (fold change) │≥ 1 and a false discovery rate (FDR) ≤0.01. The resulting p-values were adjusted using the Benjamini and Hochberg’s approach for controlling the false discovery rate. For functional prediction, the sequences were compared with the NCBI-nonredundant protein (NR) database, the Swiss-Prot protein database, and Clusters of Orthologous Groups (COG). The functional classification of the DEGs was implemented by the topGO R packages based on a Fisher’s exact test and FDR correction of 0.01, and the statistical enrichment of DEGs in KEGG pathways were performed by KOBAS software against the KEGG (Kyoto Encyclopedia of Genes and Genomes) database (http://www.kegg.jp/).

Statistical analyses

We used generalized linear models (GLM) with Tukey’s HSD in SAS (Cary, NC; SAS Institute 2000) to determine whether T. mercedesae parasitism affected flight velocity, flight distance, flight duration, homing time, and proportion of successful homing flights (p < 0.05). Mite parasitism and colony were used as independent factors. To test for any significant effects of colonies and parasitism, we inspected the colony x pathogen interaction term and therefore kept them in all models, independently of whether they were statistically significant or not. The volume of mushroom bodies was tested using one-way ANOVA followed by Tukey’s HSD (p < 0.05). Chi-square test were applied to analyze the proportion of PER by using SPSS. 22. P < 0.05 was considered statistically significant.

Results

Flight and homing ability

The flight and home ability of infested and non-infested worker bees was tested on the 15th day post emergence, and each bee flew only once. Control bees flew for 0.84 ± 0.16 h and covered 1.87 ± 0.39 km at an average velocity of 2.18 ± 0.18 km/h. T. mercedesae-infested bees flew for 0.26 ± 0.05 h and covered 0.44±0.08 km at an average velocity of 1.90 ± 0.20 km/h. Total flight distances (Fig 1A, GLM, colony: P = 0.8475, treatment: P = 0.0026, colony x treatment: P = 0.0102) and flight durations (Fig 1B, GLM, colony: P = 0.4861, treatment: P = 0.0019, colony x treatment: P = 0.0061) was significantly reduced by T. mercedesae infestation. Neve rtheless, no significant difference of the mean flight velocity was observed between T. mercedesae-infested worker bees and control bees (Fig 1C, GLM, colony: p = 0.0856, treatment: P = 0.4643, colony x treatment: P = 0.1705).

thumbnail
Fig 1. The effects of T. mercedesae infestation on flight ability of adult honey bee (n = 45).

A: flight distance; B: flight duration; C: mean flight velocity. CK: non-infested honey bees; T: T. mercedesae-infested honey bees. Horizontally, the width of each violin box represents the density of the data values. The white dots represent the median values of each group. The upper and lower edges of the black thick line represent the 3/4 digits and 1/4 digits of the data. The upper and lower ends of the thin line represent the maximum and minimum values of non-outliers of the data.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.ppat.1009684.g001

The time taken by the infested bees from releasing to arriving at home was 294.53 ± 35.69 s, while that for the non-infested worker bees was 203.29 ± 17.47 s (Fig 2, GLM, colony: P = 0.0685, treatment: P = 0.0074, colony x treatment: P = 0.0083). However, there was no significant difference in the proportion of successful homing flights between the worker bees infested with T. mercedesae (57.9%) and control bees (70.7%) (χ2 = 1.666, P = 0.197). In addition, the survival rate of honey bees infested with T. mercedesae was significantly lower than that of control bees after being put back into the colony (χ2 = 4.960, P = 0.026, S3 Fig).

thumbnail
Fig 2. The effect of T. mercedesae infestation on homing ability of adult honey bee.

Violin plot shows the homing time in control bees and T. mercedesae infested bees (n = 56–96). Horizontally, the width of each violin box represents the density of the data values. The white dots represent the median values of each group. The upper and lower edges of the black thick line represent the 3/4 digits and 1/4 digits of the data. The upper and lower ends of the thin line represent the maximum and minimum values of non-outliers of the data. CK: non-infested honey bees; T: T. mercedesae-infested honey bees.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.ppat.1009684.g002

Sucrose responsiveness and olfactory learning performance

Next, we investigated the effects of T. mercedesae on the sucrose responsiveness during worker bee development. As shown in Fig 3A, there was no significant difference in sucrose responsiveness between worker bees infested and non-infested with T. mercedesae on 0 day (χ2 = 1.816, P = 0.178), 5th day (χ2 = 3.086, P = 0.0790), and 10th day (χ2 = 0.195, P = 0.659) after emergence. But on the 15th day, the PER performance of healthy worker bees was significantly higher than that of T. mercedesae-infested worker bees (χ2 = 151.467, P < 0.010).

thumbnail
Fig 3. The effect of T. mercedesae infestation on olfactory associated functions of adult honeybee.

A. The effect of T. mercedesae infestation on sucrose responsiveness of adult honeybee. Infested and control bees were tested for PER to 30% (w/w) sucrose solutions at 0, 5, 10 and 15 days after emergence. PER rate (%) was significantly lower in infested bees at day 15 than that of control group. 0 day indicates newly emerged adult bees. B. PER responses of infested or non-infested honeybees during the conditioning phase (C1–C3). C. PER responses of infested or non-infested honeybees during the extinction phase (T1–T5). CK: non-infested honey bees; T: T. mercedesae-infested honey bees. Data are means of three independent experiments, and error bars represent ± standard error (SE). Significant differences to CK with P < 0.01 are indicated by asterisks according to chi-squared test.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.ppat.1009684.g003

In both control and infested group, the percentage of bees responding to the odor stimuli increased with the number of conditioned trials and decreased with the number of unrewarded trials. In the 1st trial (C1) and the 2nd trial (C2) of the conditioning phase, the olfactory learning performances did not change by T. mercedesae infestation (P > 0.05). At the end of the conditioning period (C3), the PER of infested worker bees was significantly lower than that of non-infested bees (χ2 = 9.639, P = 0.002, Fig 3B). During testing phase, the responses of control bees decreased from 100% (T1) to approximately 40% (T5), while only about 20% of the infested bees responded to linalool at the end of the testing period (T5). Worker bees infested with T. mercedesae showed a significantly lower response rate than that of non-infested bees in the 4th trials of the extinction phase (T4) (χ2 = 8.167, P = 0.004, Fig 3C).

Illumina sequencing and transcriptome assembly

Given the behavioral evidence above, we surmised that T. mercedesae infestation have a negative impact on the olfactory associated function of honey bees. We therefore analyzed the differences in transcripts between T. mercedesae-infested (T) and non-infested honey bees (CK) during the olfactory learning process with high-throughput RNA-seq analysis. A graphic overview of the experimental design is shown in Fig 4.

thumbnail
Fig 4. Schematic of RNA-seq experimental design.

The experimental design consisted of three groups displaying different sucrose responsiveness or learning ability.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.ppat.1009684.g004

The major sequencing assembly information is summarized in S1 Table. In total of 118 Gb sequencing data of raw reads were obtained from 18 cDNA libraries. After filtering the raw reads, approximately 51.53 million clean reads were generated, with an average GC content of 39.73%. The Q30 in each library was above 92.81%. In total, 10,141 genes (including 1,659 new genes) were identified by sequencing analysis. Over 88% of the genes were shared in all the groups from both T. mercedesae-infested and non-infested honey bees, representing 74% of the known honeybee genes [38].

DEGs between T. mercedesae-infested and non-infested honey bees

A summary of the overall changes in gene expression in various comparison in mite-infested honey bees and non-infested honey bees is shown in S2 Table. Compared with the respective controls, 86, 8, and 11 DEGs were obtained in the T. mercedesae-infested groups according to different sucrose responsiveness and learning and memory statuses. Only one shared gene, which encodes venom acid phosphatase Acph-1, was observed in the CKN/TN and CKL/TL comparisons (Fig 5A). The pairwise comparisons showed that there were very few differentially expressed genes identified in the comparison of CKN/TN and CKSN/TSN, reflecting bees without PER nor learning and memory ability have similarities in expression patterns in both infected group and control group. We further focused on the transcriptome changes of bees with learning ability between the control group and the infected group (CKL vs. TL). Compared to the CK group, we found that 50 genes were uniquely upregulated, while 36 genes were downregulated in T. mercedesae-infested bees (Fig 5B). Based on the functional annotation, a total of six unigenes were annotated as being involved in responses to environmental stimulation. Except for NF-kappa-B inhibitor (GB46554), the other five DEGs including genes encoding antimicrobial peptides (GB51223, GB47318, GB47546), potassium voltage-gated channel protein (GB43655) and major royal jelly protein 1 (GB55205) were up-regulated in TL (S2 Table). In addition, a peptidoglycan-recognition protein (GB47805) and a dolichyl-phosphate beta-glucosyltransferase (GB55419) which have been reported positively correlated with Varroa sensitive hygiene [39,40], were also upregulated in TL. Then, DEGs were aligned to the COG database to predict and classify their possible functions (Fig 5C). Eight genes were abundant in “Carbohydrate transport and metabolism”. Except for one gene encoding glyceraldehyde-3-phosphate dehydrogenase (GB50901), and other genes were upregulated compared to their levels in the CK group, including genes encoding major royal jelly proteins (GB55205, GB55208, GB55207), alpha-amylase precursor (GB49854), and synaptic vesicle glycoprotein 2C (GB49708) (S3 Table). Neurochondrin is a novel cytoplasmic protein that acts as a negative regulator of Ca2+/calmodulin-dependent protein kinase II phosphorylation and is essential for the learning process in mammals [41]. A gene encoding neurochondrin homolog protein (GB45484) was greatly downregulated (28.8-fold) in mite-infested honey bees that showed olfactory learning ability.

thumbnail
Fig 5. The differentially expressed genes (DEGs) between healthy bees and infested bees.

A. The Venn diagram of differentially expressed genes between bees with and without olfactory learning ability in both the T. mercedesae-treated group and the CK group. B. the number of DEGs in each comparison. Up-regulated and down-regulated means that these genes were higher or lower expressed in infested group compared to CK group. C. COG Function Classification of DEGs in comparision CKL/TL.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.ppat.1009684.g005

DEGs identified between bees with different sucrose responsiveness statuses

We also investigated the gene expression in bees with different sucrose responsiveness statuses in both T. mercedesae-infested group and CK group. To make the subsequent analysis more concise, CKL and TL were selected as representative groups that bees having sucrose responsiveness given that there were almost no differences in gene expression observed in the comparison of CKL vs. CKN (19 DEGs) and TL vs. TN (11 DEGs). More down-regulated genes were identified in bees with sucrose responsiveness compared to those without sucrose responsiveness, suggesting that bees tend to suppress some related genes instead of activating genes in odorant detection process (S4A Fig). The Venn diagram showed that a large number of DEG in CKSN vs. CKL were also identified in the comparison TSN vs.TL (S4B and S4C Fig). 188 and 510 DEGs were commonly up-regulated or down-regulated among all comparisons, such as genes involved in defense response (e.g. ABC transporters (GB55378, GB55375, GB41616, GB50101)), signal transduction (e.g. Serine/threonine-protein kinase (GB41700, GB53414, GB51427, GB44092, GB48061)), and carbohydrate metabolism (e.g. facilitated trehalose transporters (GB43800, GB41742, GB40972, GB54123, GB47931)). We found 120 and 666 genes were uniquely up-regulated, while 198, and 1003 genes were exclusively down-regulated expressed in each comparison, respectively (S4 Table). The GO term analysis showed that the most abundant terms were highly similar with respect to GO terms in comparison CKSN/CKL and TSN/TL (Fig 6A and 6B). The terms of “binding” (GO:0005488), “catalytic activity” (GO:0003824), “metabolic process” (GO:0008152), and “cell” (GO:0005623) are dominant. While compared to the whole genome background, GO annotation associated with biological adhesion (GO:0022610) was most significantly enriched in both CK and infested group (Q value < 0.05) (S5 and S6 Tables). Interestingly, genes related to cell adhesion, such as neural-cadherin, fat-like cadherin and protocadherin, were all down-regulated in bees with sucrose responsiveness in both CK and infested group (Fig 6C and S8 Table). Additionally, a total of 20 genes involved in sensory system showed clear separation between bees with different sucrose responsiveness statuses, especially odorant binding proteins (OBPs) and odorant receptors (ORs) (Fig 6D and S7 Table). The expression levels of most OBPs and ORs in bees having PER were significantly lower than that in bees without sucrose responsiveness. Particularly, chemosensory protein 1 (GB43823) and an odorant binding protein (GB46224) were exclusively up-regulated in TL groups.

thumbnail
Fig 6. The differentially expressed genes (DEGs) between bees with or without learning ability in CK group and T. mercedesae-treated group.

A&B, GO term enrichment analysis in CKSN/CKL and TSN/TL was performed. The first lap indicates top 20 GO term and the number of the genes corresponds to the outer lap. The second lap indicates the number of the genes in the genome background and Q values for enrichment of the DEGs for the specified biological process. The third lap indicates the ratio of the upregulated genes (deep purple) and downregulated genes (light purple). The fourth lap indicates the enrichment factor of each GO term. C. Heatmap of related transcripts identified in the comparison between bees with or without learning ability. D. Heatmap of chemosensory-related transcripts identified in the comparison between bees with or without learning ability. The colours indicate the log2-transformed expression values, which represent the expression level of a transcript identified in control group or infested group.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.ppat.1009684.g006

Morphological alterations in the antenna and mushroom bodies

Further, we investigated the morphological changes of antenna and mushroom bodies, as they are thought to be the most closely organs related to olfactory function in honey bee. By scanning the right antenna of adult honey bee during the development process with scanning electron microscopy, there is no significant differences in the number or size of olfactory cells in the 4th, 7th, and 8th right antennae between T. mercedesae-infested and non-infested bees (Fig 7A–7C).

thumbnail
Fig 7. Flagellum of the honey bee antenna observed with scanning electron microscopy (SEM).

A. SEM of antennal segments of scape for worker showing antenna and olfactory sensillum B. The number of sensilla placodea on 4, 7, 8 segments in the right antennae of infested and non-infested bees at 0, 5, 10 and 15 days. C. The size of sensilla placodea in the right antennae of infested and non-infested bees 0, 5, 10 and 15 days. For the actual measurement was performed in duplicate (i.e. two biological replicates, A and B) alongside controls.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.ppat.1009684.g007

Morphological alterations in the mushroom bodies of worker honey bees infested with T. mercedesae were observed (Fig 8A). The mean thickness of mushroom bodies in T. mercedesae-infested honey bees was 176.33 ± 15.81 μm, which was significantly thicker than the mushroom bodies in non-infested honey bees (93.89 ± 15.82 μm; F = 13.59, P = 0.02; Fig 8B).

thumbnail
Fig 8. The effect of T. mercedesae infestation on mushroom body in the brain of adult honey bees.

A. Photomicrographs of sections of mushroom body of control worker bees and T. mercedesae infested bees stained with hematoxylin and eosin. The black circle was indicated mushroom body of individual honeybee. B. The thickness of mushroom bodies of control bees and T. mercedesae infested bees. Data are means of three biological replicates, and error bars represent ± standard error (SE) (n = 3). Significant differences to CK with P < 0.05 are indicated by an asterisk above the bars and were determined by one-way ANOVA followed by Tukey’s HSD.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.ppat.1009684.g008

Discussion

In recent years, T. mercedesae infestation has severely damaged A. mellifera colonies in China, and these infestations currently threaten the whole beekeeping industry [42]. Compared to Varroa destructor, T. mercedesae is more dangerous to A. mellifera because of its short life cycle and phoretic stage, which contribute to rapid population development in A. mellifera colonies [13]. The effects of the parasite might operate within comparatively short time after a mite has entered a bee. Although the the damage caused by T. mercedesae mites are mainly in pupae and larvae stage, the negative effects may last to adult stages, with remarkably higher numbers of wounds and virus infection observing on adult honey bees [17]. In this study, we found T. mercedesae infestation negative effected on sucrose responsiveness, olfactory learning, flight and homing ability in worker bees. In addition, we identified genes related to carbohydrate transport and metabolism, stress response of xenobiotics, and neuronal function were differentially expressed between infested bees and healthy bees. Some of these genes have previously been reported as potential predictors of resistance to mite infestation [39]. To our knowledge, this is the first investigation of the molecular mechanism underlying the olfactory dysfunction caused by T. mercedesae infestation in adult honeybees.

Our results from the flight mills showed that infested bees had a significantly lower flight duration (Fig 1A) and flight distance (Fig 1B) than the control group, but no significant difference was observed between the control and infested bees for the mean velocity (Fig 1C). Previous studies have demonstrated that parasitic Varroa mites can impair the mitochondria in flight muscles and negative influence flight duration and homing ability [20,43]. By contrast, honey bees infested with another common bee pathogen, Nosema ceranae, displayed a higher flight activity which is correlated to the higher level of ethyl oleate in the parasitized bees [44]. One of the honeybees’ adaptation for flight is dense packing of enzymes for carbohydrate catabolism [45]. We found that eight genes involved in “Carbohydrate transport and metabolism” (GO:) showed significantly different expression between infested and non-infested bees (S3 Table). Glyceraldehyde-3-phosphate dehydrogenase is a key enzyme that catalyzes an important energy-yielding step in carbohydrate metabolism. In this study, the down-regulation of a gene encoding glyceraldehyde-3-phosphate dehydrogenase was observed in the heads of infested bees with learning ability (TL) suggests a lower supply of carbohydrate resources as well as a lower energy supply (Fig 5C). The energy changes may not only occur in the head but also in the whole body of bees. Whether the impairment of the flight ability by T. mercedesae infestation observed in this study is directly correlated to the identified responsive genes involved in proximate mechanisms underlying the energy supply needs further evidence for validation. The weaken olfactory learning and flying ability can cause a delay in honey bee homing behavior. We found that T. mercedesae infested-honey bees were significantly reduced in homing proportion and increased homing time versus control group (Fig 2). As with the Varroa mites [20] and insecticides [31,46], it appears that the interruption of homing behavior during the foraging process caused by T. mercedesae infestation will eventually leads to colony failure if bees lose flexibility in their response to colony demands.

The different performance in olfaction may account for the difference in the tolerance of bee to mite parasitism. Bees with higher olfactory sensitivity will initiate hygiene behaviors earlier under the low stimulation intensity of Varroa extracts, thus they can accurately detect and eliminate abnormal broods and nestmates [47,48]. In this study, the PER performance to stimulation with 30% sucrose solution of infested bees was significantly lower than that of healthy worker bees (Fig 3A). Additionally, T. mercedesae infestation depressed olfactory learning and memory ability in honey bees (Fig 3B and 3C). These results are consistent with previous studies on V. destructor [49]. It has been demonstrated that V. destructor parasitization leads to specific impairments in sucrose responsiveness and associative olfactory learning in honeybees, which may be caused by DWV infection boosted by V. destructor infestation [23].

Along with the behavioral changes induced by T. mercedesae infestation in honeybees, changes in gene expression related to physiological responses were also observed in the present study. The transcriptional expression patterns of honey bees without sucrose responsiveness (CKSN, TSN) or without learning ability (CKN, TN) were similar between the infested honey and non-infested honey (Fig 4). Several immunity genes showed higher expression level in T. mercedesae-infested bees (TL) than those in corresponding control group (CKL), including antimicrobial peptides (AMPs) (abaecin precursor (GB47318), apidaecins type 14 precursor (GB47546), hymenoptaecin preproprotein GB51223) and a cytochrome P450 (GB40288) (S2 Table). As key components of the Toll pathway, the AMPs have been regularly reported as responding to parasitic attacks by Nosema or Varroa [5052]. The defense response of bees to bacteria occurs through the rapid overexpression of AMPs, which are effectively delivered to the site of infection of natural pathogens and parasites [53,54]. An in vitro study showed that Varroa parasitism resulted in significantly higher transcript abundance for antimicrobial peptides in developing worker bees [55]. The feeding activity of mites can increase pathogenicity of the virus through immunosuppression of bee host [52,56,57]. Recently, Wu et al. described the tripartite interactions between honey bee pupae, T. mercedesae, and DWV and demonstrated that the expression levels of Defensin-1 and Hymenoptaecin are induced by DWV replication and T. mercedesae [58]. They found that the expression of hymenoptaecin in honeybee was negatively correlated with the mite vitellogenin (Vg) gene, which is essential for the reproduction of mite. It is known that neonicotinoid exposure activated NF-κB and subsequently affects the induction of antimicrobial peptides [59]. In this study, a gene encoding an NF-kappa-B inhibitor (GB46554) was found to be down-regulated in T. mercedesae-infested bees. The positive impact on NF-κB activation reflected the stress responses of honey bees to the parasitization by regulating immune-related genes, which represent the downstream effectors activated by Toll pathways [60]. Peptidoglycan recognition proteins (PGRPs) detect Lys-type peptidoglycan (PG) from gram-positive bacteria, which leads to the activation of Toll signaling pathway, and ultimately increase the synthesis of an array of potent antimicrobial peptides by the fat body [61]. Previous studies have shown that the elevated expression levels for PGRPs are associated with Varroa infestations in larvae [26,39]. In this study, we found that the transcript level of a Peptidoglycan Recognition Protein (PGRPs) family member (GB47805) was up-regulated in T. mercedesae-infested bees. Our findings are in line with existing reports in which exposure to parasitic mites or arguably to viruses and other microbes carried by mites affect immunity traits in honey bees.

MRJPs are also a family of antimicrobial peptides that protect royal jelly from bacterial infection via forming short digestion products of MRJP. The mrjp 1 precursor (GB55205), MRJP5 (GB55208), MRJ6 (GB55207) were significantly up-regulated in the heads of infested honey bees that exhibited olfactory learning than those in control group (S3 Table). MRJP1 is an important multifunctional protein in the brain of honey bees. Honey bees with weak learning ability exhibited low expression levels of mrjp 1 in Kenyon cells in the mushroom bodies [62,63]. Moreover, neonicotinoid pesticides depressed the expression of MRJP genes in honey bees brains, thereby impaired olfactory learning in honey bees [64]. Our finding may provide further evidence for the role of MRJPs in the development of learning ability of honeybees. Ca2+/calmodulin-dependent protein kinase II (CaMKII) has been proposed as the integral component in mediating long-term memory formation that is specific for the learned odor in honeybees mushroom bodies [65]. Neurochondrin is a novel cytoplasmic protein that acts as a negative regulator of Ca2+/calmodulin-dependent protein kinase II (CaMKII) phosphorylation and is essential for the learning process in mammals [41]. We found that a gene encoding neurochondrin homolog protein (GB45484) was greatly downregulated (28.8-fold) in mite-infested honey bees that showed olfactory learning ability. Activation of CaMKII leads to enhancement of synaptic transmission and provides a tag to confer stimulus specificity as well as supporting natural odor preference learning [66]. Our results implied that the infested bees required higher CaMKII activity level in order to form learning memory. Obviously, whether the transcription level of neurochondrin is correlated to the protein level as well as the activity level needs to be proved in the future.

So far, the specific mechanism of action on olfactory performance remains unclear. In this study, we also analyzed the gene expression changes between bees with different sucrose responsiveness statuses. Previous studies have demonstrated a general down-regulation of protein-coding genes after associative olfactory learning in A. mellifera [67,68]. In this study, more down-regulated genes were identified in bees showing sucrose responsiveness in both CK and infested group, indicating that the activation of some related genes occurred in olfactory dysfunction (S4 Fig). We found that in both infested group and CK group, the vast majority of gene alterations occurred in the comparison between bee with and without PER, no matter having learning ability or not (S4 Fig). It implied that changes in overall gene expression in the honeybee brain was occurred after sucrose stimulation rather than memory formation. More DEGs were identified in the infested group than that in the CK group, which reflected the intensification of internal disorders and the insufficient resilience caused by the infestation. According to GO classification, the most significantly enriched term in both CK and infested group was associated with biological adhesion (GO:0022610) (Fig 6A and 6B). The cell adhesion molecule gene such as cadherin (GB47118, GB40703), neural-cadherin (GB45972, GB45970), protocadherin (GB53331, GB49977, GB51276) which play an important role in olfaction, were down-regulated in the bees with sucrose responsiveness (Fig 6C and S8 Table). Additionally, the cadherin-associated protein, catenin (GB44972) was also down-regulated in bees with normal olfactory sensory. Parker et al. have demonstrated that bees performing rapid hygiene express up-regulation of the Down syndrome cell adhesion molecule genes, which appeared to be involved in Varroa resistance [39]. The catenin-mediated cadherin adhesion may affect the targeting of olfactory sensory neuron by restricting axons to the outer olfactory nerve layer until they reach the appropriate domain of the olfactory bulb [69]. Here, we propose that modulation of cell adhesion molecule genes reflects their engagement in olfactory conditioning and PER of the bees. The abnormal olfactory performance in honeybees are possibly caused by the abnormal intracellular signal transduction of neuronal communication in electrical synapses via activation of cadherin systems, and needs further evidence for validation [70]. The odorant binding proteins (OBPs) and odorant receptors (ORs) identified in this study showed clear separation between bees with different sucrose responsiveness statuses, no differences were observed between infested group and control group (Fig 6D). Unexpectedly, the expression levels of most odor binding proteins and odorant receptors in bees with normal olfactory sensory were significantly lower than those of bees with olfactory dysfunction in both CK and infection groups (Fig 7A). Only a chemosensory protein 1 (GB43823) was up-regulated in bees with normal olfactory performance. The down-regulation of these candidate genes do not seem to support a higher olfactory sensitivity in bees with sucrose responsiveness. These results implied that the dysfunctional olfactory behaviors may require multiple molecular mechanisms underlying the olfactory recognition. Many studies have implied that changes in gene expression are not frequently correlated to the protein level [71]. It is difficult to establish a direct correlation between the differences in gene expression in transcriptomics and physiological functions. Whether the suppression of chemosensory related genes reported herein related to the sensitive olfactory sensation is questionable and requires further study.

The mushroom bodies (MB) are central brain neuropils that are fundamentally involved in learning and memory in bees [72]. Previous findings showing that hydroxyurea treatment induced ablation of MB by volume changes and by changes in protein expression, which are considered to play a role in synaptic plasticity, learning, and memory [73]. The ablation of MB neuropil may cause an increase in the volume of the lateral calyxin within the same brain side and thereby altered the arborization pattern of olfactory projection neurons [74]. We found that the mean thickness of mushroom bodies in infested honey bees was significantly thicker than that in non-infested honey bees (Fig 8B). The induction of precocious foraging in honey bees also accelerated the expansion of mushroom body neuropil volume [75]. We speculated that T. mercedesae infestation would promote early development in honey bees and that the rapid neuron growth would promote the thickening of the mushroom bodies in honey bees. The rapid growth of neurons also accelerates apoptosis in the neuronal system, which is an important reason for the abnormal sugar sensitivity of infested honey bees. Further studies are required to confirm the identified morphological alterations in mushroom body involved in proximate learning mechanisms underlying the defense response to T. mercedesae parasitization.

The antenna are the main chemosensory detection organs in honey bees. The antennal lobes contain the primary olfactory neuropil and are connected to the promotor area in the protocerebrum and to the mushroom bodies [76]. Letzkus et al. demonstrated that the right antenna of honey bees responds better to odor learning than the left antenna [34]; additionally, the mean number of sensilla placodea is significantly higher in the right antenna than in the left antenna [77]. In this study, we tested the number and size of olfactory cells in the 4th, 7th, and 8th right antennae (Fig 7A–7C). The data showed that T. mercedesae infestation did not affect number or sensilla placodea cell size of the right antennae, suggesting that observed learning and memory impairment in this study may be not related to the antenna morphology.

Altogether, those findings provide novel insights into the response of host honey bees to T. mercedesae infection. Our data highlight a series of behavioral changes associated with T. mercedesae infection in adult honey bees. In this study, we found that T. mercedesae infestation affected the olfactory performance of honeybees, which is potentially caused by changes in gene expression involved in immune systems, carbohydrate transport and metabolism, and regulation of CaMKII activity. In addition, we found genes function in cell adhesion play an essential role in olfactory sensory in honey bees. Whether the transcription level of these genes is correlated to the protein level needs to be proved in the future. Further studies are required to confirm the identified responsive genes involved in proximate mechanisms underlying the resistance to T. mercedesae.

Supporting information

S1 Fig. The flight mill used to test the flight ability of tethered forager bees.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.ppat.1009684.s001

(TIF)

S2 Fig. Apis mellifera infested with Tropilaelaps mercedesae (untreated control) used for the PER paradigm.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.ppat.1009684.s002

(TIF)

S3 Fig. The survival rate of bees after homing ability test.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.ppat.1009684.s003

(TIF)

S4 Fig. The differentially expressed genes (DEGs) between honey bees with different sucrose responsiveness in control group or infested group.

A. Histogram of the number of DEGs identified in various comparisons. B. Venn diagram of up-regulated DEGs in various comparisons. C. Venn diagram of down-regulated DEGs in various comparisons. DEGs indicate that these genes were expressed more or less in latter group group than in the corresponding former group.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.ppat.1009684.s004

(TIF)

S1 Table. Summary of the Illumina transcriptome assembly for samples tested in this study.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.ppat.1009684.s005

(XLSX)

S2 Table. Information of DEGs between mite-infested and non-mite infested honeybees associated with different sucrose responsiveness and learning statuses.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.ppat.1009684.s006

(XLSX)

S3 Table. DEGS are abundant in Carbohydrate transport and metabolism in comparison CKL vsTL.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.ppat.1009684.s007

(XLSX)

S4 Table. Summary of DEGS identified in comparison of CKSN vs. CKL and TSN vs. TL.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.ppat.1009684.s008

(XLSX)

S5 Table. GO classification of theDEGs identified in comparison CKSN vs. CKL.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.ppat.1009684.s009

(XLSX)

S6 Table. GO classification of theDEGs identified in comparison TSN vs. TL.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.ppat.1009684.s010

(XLSX)

S7 Table. DEGS that assigned as chemosensory genes identified in various comparisons.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.ppat.1009684.s011

(XLSX)

S8 Table. DEGS that associated to bioadhesion identified in various comparisons.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.ppat.1009684.s012

(XLSX)

Acknowledgments

We thank Yujuan Qiu and Guirong Li (Institute of Apicultural Research, China Academy of Agricultural Sciences) for beekeeping.

References

  1. 1. Potts SG, Biesmeijer JC, Kremen C, Neumann P, Schweiger O, Kunin WE. Global pollinator declines: trends, impacts and drivers. Trends in ecology & evolution. 2010;25(6):345–53. Epub 2010/03/02. pmid:20188434.
  2. 2. Piou V, Tabart J, Urrutia V, Hemptinne JL, Vétillard A. Impact of the Phoretic Phase on Reproduction and Damage Caused by Varroa destructor (Anderson and Trueman) to Its Host, the European Honey Bee (Apis mellifera L.). PLoS One. 2016;11(4):e0153482. Epub 2016/04/21. pmid:27096154; PubMed Central PMCID: PMC4838260.
  3. 3. Ramsey SD, Ochoa R, Bauchan G, Gulbronson C, Mowery JD, Cohen A, et al. Varroa destructor feeds primarily on honey bee fat body tissue and not hemolymph. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2019;116(5):1792–801. Epub 2019/01/17. pmid:30647116; PubMed Central PMCID: PMC6358713.
  4. 4. Khongphinitbunjong K, Neumann P, Chantawannakul P, Williams GR. The ectoparasitic mite Tropilaelaps mercedesae reduces western honey bee, Apis mellifera, longevity and emergence weight, and promotes Deformed wing virus infections. Journal of Invertebrate Pathology. 2016;137:38–42. WOS:000378300800005. pmid:27126517
  5. 5. Khongphinitbunjong K, de Guzman LI, Burgett MD, Rinderer TE, Chantawannakul P. Behavioral responses underpinning resistance and susceptibility of honeybees to Tropilaelaps mercedesae. Apidologie. 2012;43(5):590–9. WOS:000310226400010.
  6. 6. Chantawannakul P, Ramsey S, vanEngelsdorp D, Khongphinitbunjong K, Phokasem P. Tropilaelaps mite: an emerging threat to European honey bee. Current Opinion in Insect Science. 2018;26:69–75. WOS:000432522700012. pmid:29764663
  7. 7. Anderson DL, Morgan MJ. Genetic and morphological variation of bee-parasitic Tropilaelaps mites (Acari: Laelapidae): new and re-defined species. Experimental and Applied Acarology. 2007;43(1):1–24. CABI:20093308906. pmid:17828576
  8. 8. Buawangpong N, de Guzman LI, Khongphinitbunjong K, Frake AM, Burgett M, Chantawannakul P. Prevalence and reproduction of Tropilaelaps mercedesae and Varroa destructor in concurrently infested Apis mellifera colonies. Apidologie. 2015;46(6):779–86. WOS:000364911900010.
  9. 9. de Guzman LI, Williams GR, Khongphinitbunjong K, Chantawannakul P. Ecology, Life History, and Management of Tropilaelaps Mites. Journal of Economic Entomology. 2017;110(2):319–32. WOS:000401468700003. pmid:28334185
  10. 10. Pettis JS, Rose R, Lichtenberg EM, Chantawannakul P, Buawangpong N, Somana W, et al. A rapid survey technique for Tropilaelaps mite (Mesostigmata: Laelapidae) detection. J Econ Entomol. 2013;106(4):1535–44. Epub 2013/09/12. pmid:24020263
  11. 11. Anderson DL, Roberts JMK. Standard methods for Tropilaelaps mites research. Journal of Apicultural Research. 2013;52(4). WOS:000323845800019.
  12. 12. Rinderer TE, Oldroyd BP, Lekprayoon C, Wongsiri S, Boonthai C, Thapa R. Extended survival of the parasitic honey bee mite Tropilaelaps clareae on adult workers of Apis mellifera and Apis dorsata. Journal of Apicultural Research. 1994;33(3):171–4. CABI:19940201225.
  13. 13. Forsgren E, de Miranda JR, Isaksson M, Wei S, Fries I. Deformed wing virus associated with Tropilaelaps mercedesae infesting European honey bees (Apis mellifera). Experimental and Applied Acarology. 2009;47(2):87–97. WOS:000262087700001. pmid:18941909
  14. 14. Nazzi F, Pennacchio F. Honey Bee Antiviral Immune Barriers as Affected by Multiple Stress Factors: A Novel Paradigm to Interpret Colony Health Decline and Collapse. Viruses-Basel. 2018;10(4). WOS:000435184400020. pmid:29601473
  15. 15. de Guzman LI, Simone-Finstrom M, Cervancia C, Tokarz P, Frake AM. Tropilaelaps species identification and viral load evaluation of Tropilaelaps and Varroa mites and their Apis mellifera hosts in Palawan, Philippines. Journal of Invertebrate Pathology. 2020;170. WOS:000552825100010. pmid:31926971
  16. 16. Khongphinitbunjong K, de Guzman LI, Tarver MR, Rinderer TE, Chantawannakul P. Interactions of Tropilaelaps mercedesae, honey bee viruses and immune response in Apis mellifera. Journal of Apicultural Research. 2015;54(1):40–7. WOS:000359648500005.
  17. 17. Phokasem P, de Guzman LI, Khongphinitbunjong K, Frake AM, Chantawannakul P. Feeding by Tropilaelaps mercedesae on pre- and post-capped brood increases damage to Apis mellifera colonies. Scientific Reports. 2019;9. WOS:000484988100018. pmid:31506594
  18. 18. Ma S, Yang Y, Diao Q, Fu Z, Dai P. Effects of Tropilaelaps mercedesae on the survival and food consumption of honey bees (Apis mellifera). Chinese Journal of Applied Entomology. 2018;55(6):1066–73. CSCD:6453889.
  19. 19. Ma S, Yang Y, Fu Z, Diao Q, Wang M, Luo Q, et al. A combination of Tropilaelaps mercedesae and imidacloprid negatively affects survival, pollen consumption and midgut bacterial composition of honey bee. Chemosphere. 2020;268:129368. Epub 2020/12/29. pmid:33360943
  20. 20. Kralj J, Fuchs S. Parasitic Varroa destructor mites influence flight duration and homing ability of infested Apis mellifera foragers. Apidologie. 2006;37(5):577–87. :2006040. WOS:000242177000008.
  21. 21. Duay P, Jong Dd, Engels W. Decreased flight performance and sperm production in drones of the honey bee (Apis mellifera) slightly infested by Varroa destructor mites during pupal development. Genetics and Molecular Research. 2002;1(3):227–32. CABI:20033038941. pmid:14963829
  22. 22. Bubalo D, Pechhacker H, Licek E, Kezic N, Sulimanovic D. The effect of Varroa destructor infestation on flight activity and mating efficiency of drones (Apis mellifera L.). Wiener Tierarztliche Monatsschrift. 2005;92(1):11–5. WOS:000227324300003.
  23. 23. Nazzi F, Brown SP, Annoscia D, Del Piccolo F, Di Prisco G, Varricchio P, et al. Synergistic Parasite-Pathogen Interactions Mediated by Host Immunity Can Drive the Collapse of Honeybee Colonies. Plos Pathogens. 2012;8(6). WOS:000305987800009. pmid:22719246
  24. 24. Scheiner R. Birth weight and sucrose responsiveness predict cognitive skills of honeybee foragers. Animal Behaviour. 2012;84(2):305–8. WOS:000306656000003.
  25. 25. Annoscia D, Zanni V, Galbraith D, Quirici A, Grozinger C, Bortolomeazzi R, et al. Elucidating the mechanisms underlying the beneficial health effects of dietary pollen on honey bees (Apis mellifera) infested by Varroa mite ectoparasites. Scientific Reports. 2017;7. WOS:000406260100062. pmid:28740210
  26. 26. Gregorc A, Evans JD, Scharf M, Ellis JD. Gene expression in honey bee (Apis mellifera) larvae exposed to pesticides and Varroa mites (Varroa destructor). Journal of Insect Physiology. 2012;58(8):1042–9. WOS:000307154600002. pmid:22497859
  27. 27. Zhang Y, Liu XJ, Zhang WQ, Han RC. Differential gene expression of the honey bees Apis mellifera and A. cerana induced by Varroa destructor infection. Journal of Insect Physiology. 2010;56(9):1207–18. WOS:000281087500026. pmid:20346951
  28. 28. Khongphinitbunjong K, de Guzman LI, Buawangpong N, Rinderer TE, Frake AM, Chantawannakul P. Observations on the removal of brood inoculated with Tropilaelaps mercedesae (Acari: Laelapidae) and the mite’s reproductive success in Apis mellifera colonies. Experimental and Applied Acarology. 2014;62(1):47–55. WOS:000328121700004. pmid:23979656
  29. 29. Ma S, Yang Y, Jack CJ, Diao Q, Fu Z, Dai P. Effects of Tropilaelaps mercedesae on midgut bacterial diversity of Apis mellifera. Experimental and Applied Acarology. 2019;79(2):169–86. WOS:000493608800002. pmid:31602536
  30. 30. Ma CS, Zhang YK, Sun J, Imran M, Yang HP, Wu J, et al. Impact of acute oral exposure to thiamethoxam on the homing, flight, learning acquisition and short-term retention of Apis cerana. Pest Management Science. 2019;75(11):2975–80. WOS:000488988100015. pmid:30884080
  31. 31. Tosi S, Burgio G, Nieh JC. A common neonicotinoid pesticide, thiamethoxam, impairs honey bee flight ability. Scientific Reports. 2017;7. WOS:000400058400001. pmid:28446783
  32. 32. Decourtye A, Devillers J, Genecque E, Le Menach K, Budzinski H, Cluzeau S, et al. Comparative sublethal toxicity of nine pesticides on olfactory learning performances of the honeybee Apis mellifera. Archives of Environmental Contamination and Toxicology. 2005;48(2):242–50. WOS:000227608800012. pmid:15750780
  33. 33. Strube-Bloss MF, Rossler W. Multimodal integration and stimulus categorization in putative mushroom body output neurons of the honeybee. Royal Society Open Science. 2018;5(2). WOS:000426465700070. pmid:29515886
  34. 34. Letzkus P, Ribi WA, Wood JT, Zhu H, Zhang S-W, Srinivasan MV. Lateralization of olfaction in the honeybee Apis mellifera. Curr Biol. 2006;16(14):1471–6. WOS:000239324700031. pmid:16860748
  35. 35. Estrada JC, Angelica SM, Miller SE. TEM of paraffin-embedded H&E-stained sections for viral diagnosis (an unusual papovavirus case). Microsc Microanal. 2005;13:22–5.
  36. 36. Bolger AM, Lohse M, Usadel B. Trimmomatic: a flexible trimmer for Illumina sequence data. Bioinformatics. 2014;30(15):2114–20. WOS:000340049100004. pmid:24695404
  37. 37. Mortazavi A, Williams BA, McCue K, Schaeffer L, Wold B. Mapping and quantifying mammalian transcriptomes by RNA-Seq. Nature Methods. 2008;5(7):621–8. WOS:000257166700015. pmid:18516045
  38. 38. Elsik CG, Worley KC, Bennett AK, Beye M, Camara F, Childers CP, et al. Finding the missing honey bee genes: lessons learned from a genome upgrade. Bmc Genomics. 2014;15. WOS:000331116900002. pmid:24479613
  39. 39. Parker R, Guarna MM, Melathopoulos AP, Moon KM, White R, Huxter E, et al. Correlation of proteome-wide changes with social immunity behaviors provides insight into resistance to the parasitic mite, Varroa destructor, in the honey bee (Apis mellifera). Genome Biology. 2012;13(9). WOS:000313182600010. pmid:23021491
  40. 40. Ji T, Shen F, Liu Z, Yin L, Shen J, Liang Q, et al. Comparative proteomic analysis reveals mite (Varroa destructor) resistance-related proteins in Eastern honeybees (Apis cerana). Genetics and Molecular Research. 2015;14(3):10103–18. WOS:000362421500158. pmid:26345948
  41. 41. Dateki M, Horii T, Kasuya Y, Mochizuki R, Nagao Y, Ishida J, et al. Neurochondrin negatively regulates CaMKII phosphorylation, and nervous system-specific gene disruption results in epileptic seizure. Journal of Biological Chemistry. 2005;280(21):20503–8. WOS:000229242000043. pmid:15790563
  42. 42. Luo Q-H, Zhou T, Dai P-L, Song H-L, Wu Y-Y, Wang Q. Prevalence, intensity and associated factor analysis of Tropilaelaps mercedesae infesting Apis mellifera in China. Experimental and Applied Acarology. 2011;55(2):135–46. WOS:000297627800002. pmid:21590361
  43. 43. Blanken LJ, van Langevelde F, van Dooremalen C. Interaction between Varroa destructor and imidacloprid reduces flight capacity of honeybees. Proceedings of the Royal Society B-Biological Sciences. 2015;282(1820). WOS:000368095200003. pmid:26631559
  44. 44. Dussaubat C, Maisonnasse A, Crauser D, Beslay D, Costagliola G, Soubeyrand S, et al. Flight behavior and pheromone changes associated to Nosema ceranae infection of honey bee workers (Apis mellifera) in field conditions. Journal of Invertebrate Pathology. 2013;113(1):42–51. WOS:000317372000006. pmid:23352958
  45. 45. Suarez RK, Staples JF, Lighton JRB, Mathieu-Costello O. Mitochondrial function in flying honeybees (Apis mellifera): Respiratory chain enzymes and electron flow from complex III to oxygen. Journal of Experimental Biology. 2000;203(5):905–11. WOS:000086149500007.
  46. 46. Matsumoto T. Reduction in homing flights in the honey bee Apis mellifera after a sublethal dose of neonicotinoid insecticides. Bulletin of Insectology. 2013;66(1):1–9. WOS:000319310300001.
  47. 47. Martin C, Provost E, Roux M, Bruchou C, Crauser D, Clement JL, et al. Resistance of the honey bee, Apis mellifera to the acarian parasite Varroa destructor: behavioural and electroantennographic data. Physiological Entomology. 2001;26(4):362–70. WOS:000172905200011.
  48. 48. Swanson JAI, Torto B, Kells SA, Mesce KA, Tumlinson JH, Spivak M. Odorants that Induce Hygienic Behavior in Honeybees: Identification of Volatile Compounds in Chalkbrood-Infected Honeybee Larvae. Journal of Chemical Ecology. 2009;35(9):1108–16. WOS:000270634300011. pmid:19816752
  49. 49. Kralj J, Brockmann A, Fuchs S, Tautz J. The parasitic mite Varroa destructor affects non-associative learning in honey bee foragers, Apis mellifera L. Journal of comparative physiology A, Neuroethology, sensory, neural, and behavioral physiology. 2007;193(3):363–70. Epub 2006/11/24. pmid:17123087.
  50. 50. Chaimanee V, Chantawannakul P, Chen Y, Evans JD, Pettis JS. Effects of host age on susceptibility to infection and immune gene expression in honey bee queens (Apis mellifera) inoculated with Nosema ceranae. Apidologie. 2014;45(4):451–63. WOS:000338216700006.
  51. 51. Zanni V, Galbraith DA, Annoscia D, Grozinger CM, Nazzi F. Transcriptional signatures of parasitization and markers of colony decline in Varroa-infested honey bees (Apis mellifera). Insect Biochemistry and Molecular Biology. 2017;87:1–13. WOS:000408289200001. pmid:28595898
  52. 52. Kuster RD, Boncristiani HF, Rueppell O. Immunogene and viral transcript dynamics during parasitic Varroa destructor mite infection of developing honey bee (Apis mellifera) pupae. Journal of Experimental Biology. 2014;217(10):1710–8. WOS:000338517700014. pmid:24829325
  53. 53. Bulet P, Hetru C, Dimarcq JL, Hoffmann D. Antimicrobial peptides in insects; structure and function. Developmental and Comparative Immunology. 1999; 234–5:329–44.
  54. 54. Evans JD, Lopez DL. Bacterial Probiotics induce an immune response in the honey bee (Hymenoptera: Apidae). Journal of Economic Entomology. 2004;97(3):752–6. WOS:000222315200006. pmid:15279248
  55. 55. Gregorc A, Planinc I. USE OF THYMOL FORMULATIONS, AMITRAZ, AND OXALIC ACID FOR THE CONTROL OF THE VARROA MITE IN HONEY BEE (Apis mellifera carnica) COLONIES. Journal of Apicultural Science. 2012;56(2):61–9. WOS:000312519500007.
  56. 56. Yang XL, Cox-Foster DL. Impact of an ectoparasite on the immunity and pathology of an invertebrate: Evidence for host immunosuppression and viral amplification. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America. 2005;102(21):7470–5. WOS:000229417500015. pmid:15897457
  57. 57. Gregory PG, Evans JD, Rinderer T, de Guzman L. Conditional immune-gene suppression of honeybees parasitized by Varroa mites. Journal of Insect Science. 2005;5. WOS:000228141900001. pmid:16299595
  58. 58. Wu YF, Liu QS, Weiss B, Kaltenpoth M, Kadowaki T. Honey Bee Suppresses the Parasitic Mite Vitellogenin by Antimicrobial Peptide. Frontiers in Microbiology. 2020;11. WOS:000540627300001. pmid:32523577
  59. 59. Di Prisco G, Cavaliere V, Annoscia D, Varricchio P, Caprio E, Nazzi F, et al. Neonicotinoid clothianidin adversely affects insect immunity and promotes replication of a viral pathogen in honey bees. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America. 2013;110(46):18466–71. WOS:000326830900045. pmid:24145453
  60. 60. Vieira CS, Moreira OC, Batista KKS, Ratcliffe NA, Castro DP, Azambuja P. The NF-kappa B Inhibitor, IMD-0354, Affects Immune Gene Expression, Bacterial Microbiota and Trypanosoma cruzi Infection in Rhodnius prolixus Midgut. Frontiers in Physiology. 2018;9. WOS:000443269900001. pmid:30233391
  61. 61. Gottar M, Gobert V, Michel T, Belvin M, Duyk G, Hoffmann JA, et al. The Drosophila immune response against Gram-negative bacteria is mediated by a peptidoglycan recognition protein. Nature. 2002;416(6881):640–4. WOS:000174901900045. pmid:11912488
  62. 62. Kucharski R, Maleszka R, Hayward DC, Ball EE. A royal jelly protein is expressed in a subset of Kenyon cells in the mushroom bodies of the honey bee brain. Naturwissenschaften. 1998;85(7):343–6. WOS:000075267300006. pmid:9722965
  63. 63. Hojo M, Kagami T, Sasaki T, Nakamura J, Sasaki M. Reduced expression of major royal jelly protein 1 gene in the mushroom bodies of worker honeybees with reduced learning ability. Apidologie. 2010;41(2):194–202. WOS:000276461200010.
  64. 64. Li Z, Yu T, Chen Y, Heerman M, He J, Huang J, et al. Brain transcriptome of honey bees (Apis mellifera) exhibiting impaired olfactory learning induced by a sublethal dose of imidacloprid. Pesticide Biochemistry and Physiology. 2019;156:36–43. WOS:000466822300004. pmid:31027579
  65. 65. Matsumoto Y, Sandoz J-C, Devaud J-M, Lormant F, Mizunami M, Giurfa M. Cyclic nucleotide-gated channels, calmodulin, adenylyl cyclase, and calcium/calmodulin-dependent protein kinase II are required for late, but not early, long-term memory formation in the honeybee. Learning & Memory. 2014;21(5):272–86. WOS:000338505300004. pmid:24741108
  66. 66. Moriguchi S, Oomura Y, Shioda N, Han F, Hori N, Aou S, et al. Ca2+/calmodulin-dependent protein kinase II and protein kinase C activities mediate extracellular glucose-regulated hippocampal synaptic efficacy. Molecular and Cellular Neuroscience. 2011;46(1):101–7. WOS:000286487800010. pmid:20807573
  67. 67. Cristino AS, Barchuk AR, Freitas FCP, Narayanan RK, Biergans SD, Zhao Z, et al. Neuroligin-associated microRNA-932 targets actin and regulates memory in the honeybee. Nature Communications. 2014;5. WOS:000345622500006. pmid:25409902
  68. 68. Wang Z-L, Wang H, Qin Q-H, Zeng Z-J. Gene expression analysis following olfactory learning in Apis mellifera. Molecular Biology Reports. 2013;40(2):1631–9. WOS:000313165500098. pmid:23073783
  69. 69. Akins MR, Greer CA. Axon behavior in the olfactory nerve reflects the involvement of catenin-cadherin mediated adhesion. Journal of Comparative Neurology. 2006;499(6):979–89. WOS:000242637500009. pmid:17072833
  70. 70. Tachibana K, Nakanishi H, Mandai K, Ozaki K, Ikeda W, Yamamoto Y, et al. Two cell adhesion molecules, nectin and cadherin, interact through their cytoplasmic domain-associated proteins. Journal of Cell Biology. 2000;150(5):1161–75. WOS:000089305500019. pmid:10974003
  71. 71. Vogel C, Marcotte EM. Insights into the regulation of protein abundance from proteomic and transcriptomic analyses. Nature Reviews Genetics. 2012;13(4):227–32. WOS:000301785600008. pmid:22411467
  72. 72. Zwaka H, Muench D, Manz G, Menzel R, Rybak J. The Circuitry of Olfactory Projection Neurons in the Brain of the Honeybee, Apis mellifera. Frontiers in Neuroanatomy. 2016;10. WOS:000384667000001. pmid:27746723
  73. 73. Malun D, Plath N, Giurfa M, Moseleit AD, Muller U. Hydroxyurea-induced partial mushroom body ablation in the honeybee Apis mellifera: Volumetric analysis and quantitative protein determination. Journal of Neurobiology. 2002;50(1):31–44. WOS:000172931600005. pmid:11748631
  74. 74. Ganeshina O, Menzel R. GABA-immunoreactive neurons in the mushroom bodies of the honeybee: An electron microscopic study. Journal of Comparative Neurology. 2001;437(3):335–49. WOS:000170311000006. pmid:11494260
  75. 75. Fahrbach SE, Moore D, Capaldi EA, Farris SM, Robinson GE. Experience-expectant plasticity in the mushroom bodies of the honeybee. Learning & Memory. 1998;5(1–2):115–23. WOS:000078894100008. pmid:10454376
  76. 76. Heisenberg M, Gerber B. Common design of mushroom bodies in bees and flies? Journal of Comparative Neurology. 2002;450(1):1–3. WOS:000176955500001. pmid:12124763
  77. 77. Frasnelli E, Vallortigara G. Distribution of Antennal Olfactory and Non-Olfactory Sensilla in Different Species of Bees. Symmetry-Basel. 2017;9(8). WOS:000408751100009.